The scaling behavior of hand motions reveals self-organization during an executive function task

Recent approaches to cognition explain cognitive phenomena in terms of interaction-dominant dynamics. In the current experiment, we extend this approach to executive function, a construct used to describe flexible, goal-oriented behavior. Participants were asked to perform a widely used executive function task, card sorting, under two conditions. In one condition, participants were given a rule with which to sort the cards. In the other condition, participants had to induce the rule from experimenter feedback. The motion of each participant’s hand was tracked during the sorting task. Detrended fluctuation analysis was performed on the inter-point time series using a windowing strategy to capture changes over each trial. For participants in the induction condition, the Hurst exponent sharply increased and then decreased. The Hurst exponents for the explicit condition did not show this pattern. Our results suggest that executive function may be understood in terms of changes in stability that arise from interaction-dominant dynamics.

[1]  Damian G. Stephen,et al.  The dynamics of insight: Mathematical discovery as a phase transition , 2009, Memory & cognition.

[2]  Paolo Grigolini,et al.  A theory of 1/f noise in human cognition , 2009 .

[3]  P. Anderson Assessment and Development of Executive Function (EF) During Childhood , 2002, Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence.

[4]  E. Kaplan,et al.  Comparison of two short forms of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. , 1991, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[5]  Linda B. Smith Cognition as a dynamic system: Principles from embodiment , 2005 .

[6]  A. Baddeley Exploring the Central Executive , 1996 .

[7]  David L. Gilden,et al.  Fluctuations in the Time Required for Elementary Decisions , 1997 .

[8]  H. Eugene Stanley,et al.  Switching Phenomena in a System with No Switches , 2010 .

[9]  H. Stanley,et al.  Quantification of scaling exponents and crossover phenomena in nonstationary heartbeat time series. , 1995, Chaos.

[10]  Damian G. Stephen,et al.  Dynamics of representational change: entropy, action, and cognition. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  J. Singer,et al.  Applied Longitudinal Data Analysis , 2003 .

[12]  V. Plerou,et al.  Scale invariance and universality: organizing principles in complex systems , 2000 .

[13]  Barbara Hayes-Roth,et al.  A Cognitive Model of Planning , 1979, Cogn. Sci..

[14]  Daniel Mirman,et al.  Interactions dominate the dynamics of visual cognition , 2010, Cognition.

[15]  Klaus Oberauer,et al.  Accessing information in working memory: can the focus of attention grasp two elements at the same time? , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[16]  Damian G. Stephen,et al.  The role of fractality in perceptual learning: exploration in dynamic touch. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  P. Zelazo,et al.  The development of executive function in early childhood: VI. The development of executive function: Cognitive complexity and control--revised. , 2003 .

[18]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Human memory retrieval as Lévy foraging , 2007 .

[19]  G. V. van Orden,et al.  Dispersion of response times reveals cognitive dynamics. , 2009, Psychological review.

[20]  H. Stanley,et al.  Effect of trends on detrended fluctuation analysis. , 2001, Physical review. E, Statistical, nonlinear, and soft matter physics.

[21]  Thadeu Josino Pereira Penna,et al.  Fourier-detrended fluctuation analysis , 2005 .

[22]  G. V. van Orden,et al.  Self-organization of cognitive performance. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.