Taking Stock of Unrealistic Optimism

Researchers have used terms such as unrealistic optimism and optimistic bias to refer to concepts that are similar but not synonymous. Drawing from 3 decades of research, we discuss critically how researchers define unrealistic optimism, and we identify four types that reflect different measurement approaches: unrealistic absolute optimism at the individual and group levels and unrealistic comparative optimism at the individual and group levels. In addition, we discuss methodological criticisms leveled against research on unrealistic optimism and note that the criticisms are primarily relevant to only one type: the group form of unrealistic comparative optimism. We further clarify how the criticisms are not nearly as problematic as they might seem, even for unrealistic comparative optimism. Finally, we note boundary conditions on the different types of unrealistic optimism and reflect on five broad questions that deserve further attention.

[1]  R. Dolan,et al.  How Dopamine Enhances an Optimism Bias in Humans , 2012, Current Biology.

[2]  T. Sharot The optimism bias , 2011, Current Biology.

[3]  Christoph W Korn,et al.  How unrealistic optimism is maintained in the face of reality , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[4]  Tali Sharot,et al.  The Optimism Bias: A Tour of the Irrationally Positive Brain , 2011 .

[5]  W. Klein,et al.  Unrealistic optimism in early-phase oncology trials. , 2011, IRB.

[6]  Ulrike Hahn,et al.  Unrealistic optimism about future life events: a cautionary note. , 2011, Psychological review.

[7]  Vital signs: current cigarette smoking among adults aged ≥18 years--United States, 2005-2010. , 2011, MMWR. Morbidity and mortality weekly report.

[8]  Erika A. Waters,et al.  Correlates of unrealistic risk beliefs in a nationally representative sample , 2011, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[9]  J. Shepperd,et al.  The costs of optimism and the benefits of pessimism. , 2010, Emotion.

[10]  Krista W Ranby,et al.  Perceived susceptibility measures are not interchangeable: absolute, direct comparative, and indirect comparative risk. , 2010, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[11]  J. Shepperd,et al.  Does mood really influence comparative optimism? Tracking an elusive effect. , 2009, The British journal of social psychology.

[12]  W. Klein,et al.  The Dark Side of Optimism: Unrealistic Optimism About Problems With Alcohol Predicts Subsequent Negative Event Experiences , 2009, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[13]  Theresa M Marteau,et al.  Impact of genetic risk information and type of disease on perceived risk, anticipated affect, and expected consequences of genetic tests. , 2009, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[14]  M. Helweg-Larsen,et al.  Smoking cross-culturally: Risk perceptions among young adults in Denmark and the United States , 2009, Psychology & health.

[15]  W. Klein,et al.  Imagining.a.Rosy.Future:.The.Psychology.of.Optimism , 2008 .

[16]  J. Shepperd,et al.  Comparative optimism in the spontaneous generation of future life-events. , 2008, The British journal of social psychology.

[17]  J. Shepperd,et al.  Do People Brace Sensibly? Risk Judgments and Event Likelihood , 2007, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[18]  Deborah A Small,et al.  Error and bias in comparative judgment: on being both better and worse than we think we are. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[19]  Deborah A. Small,et al.  Error and Bias in Comparative Judgment: On Being Both Better and Worse than We Think We are , 2007 .

[20]  John R. Chambers,et al.  The Rational Side of Egocentrism in Social Comparisons , 2006 .

[21]  P. Carroll,et al.  Forsaking Optimism , 2006 .

[22]  W. Klein,et al.  Unrealistic Optimism in Smokers: Implications for Smoking Myth Endorsement and Self-Protective Motivation , 2006, Journal of health communication.

[23]  W. Klein,et al.  Breast cancer risk perceptions and breast cancer worry: what predicts what? , 2005 .

[24]  J. Shepperd,et al.  Anxiety and Outcome Predictions , 2005, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[25]  Victor J. Strecher,et al.  Do cigarette smokers have unrealistic perceptions of their heart attack, cancer, and stroke risks? , 1995, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[26]  T. Revenson,et al.  Unrealistic optimism among adolescent smokers and nonsmokers , 1991, Journal of Primary Prevention.

[27]  P. Slovic,et al.  Accuracy and optimism in smokers' beliefs about quitting. , 2004, Nicotine & tobacco research : official journal of the Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco.

[28]  Justin Kruger,et al.  If you don't want to be late, enumerate: Unpacking reduces the planning fallacy , 2004 .

[29]  John R. Chambers,et al.  Biases in social comparative judgments: the role of nonmotivated factors in above-average and comparative-optimism effects. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.

[30]  Jeremy Burrus,et al.  Egocentrism and focalism in unrealistic optimism (and pessimism) , 2004 .

[31]  G. Colditz,et al.  Colon Cancer: Risk Perceptions and Risk Communication , 2004, Journal of health communication.

[32]  F. Gibbons,et al.  Perceptions of smoking risk as a function of smoking status , 1992, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[33]  Christina Lee Perceptions of immunity to disease in adult smokers , 1989, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[34]  N. Weinstein Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems: Conclusions from a community-wide sample , 1987, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[35]  Office on Smoking The Health Consequences of Smoking: A Report of the Surgeon General , 2004 .

[36]  Jerry Suls,et al.  Egocentrism, Event Frequency, and Comparative Optimism: When what Happens Frequently is “More Likely to Happen to Me” , 2003, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[37]  Marie Helweg-Larsen,et al.  Are Comparative Risk Judgments Consistent Across Time and Events? , 2003, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[38]  Justin Kruger,et al.  The influence of egocentrism and focalism on people's optimism in competitions: when what affects us equally affects me more. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[39]  J. Kleijnen,et al.  Context Effects , 2003 .

[40]  W. Klein,et al.  Dispositional, Unrealistic, and Comparative Optimism: Differential Relations with the Knowledge and Processing of Risk Information and Beliefs about Personal Risk , 2002 .

[41]  P. C. Price,et al.  Perceived Event Frequency and the Optimistic Bias: Evidence for a Two-Process Model of Personal Risk Judgments ☆ , 2002 .

[42]  Marie Helweg-Larsen,et al.  Perceived Control and the Optimistic Bias: A Meta-Analytic Review , 2002 .

[43]  James A. Shepperd,et al.  Exploring the causes of comparative optimism. , 2002 .

[44]  Thomas G. Hungar IN THE UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE DISTRICT OF COLUMBIA , 2002 .

[45]  Marie Helweg-Larsen,et al.  Do Moderators of the Optimistic Bias Affect Personal or Target Risk Estimates? A Review of the Literature , 2001 .

[46]  Jennifer S. Beer,et al.  Positive illusions about the self: short-term benefits and long-term costs. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[47]  J. Eiser,et al.  Optimism, Pessimism, and the Direction of Self–Other Comparisons , 2001 .

[48]  N. Epley,et al.  Feeling "holier than thou": are self-serving assessments produced by errors in self- or social prediction? , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[49]  Sander L. Koole,et al.  Overcoming the planning fallacy through willpower : effects of implementation intentions on actual and predicted task-completion times , 2000 .

[50]  Derek J. Koehler,et al.  People focus on optimistic scenarios and disregard pessimistic scenarios while predicting task completion times. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[51]  Richard Joiner,et al.  The typical student as an in‐group member: eliminating optimistic bias by reducing social distance , 2000 .

[52]  R. W. Rogers,et al.  A meta-analysis of research on protection motivation theory. , 2000 .

[53]  C W Dent,et al.  Perceived invulnerability and cigarette smoking among adolescents. , 2000, Addictive behaviors.

[54]  M. Ross,et al.  Assessing the Accuracy of Predictions about Dating Relationships: How and Why Do Lovers’ Predictions Differ from those Made by Observers? , 1999 .

[55]  Neil D. Weinstein,et al.  Mindset, optimistic bias about personal risk and health-protective behaviour , 1999 .

[56]  J. Kruger Lake Wobegon be gone! The "below-average effect" and the egocentric nature of comparative ability judgments. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[57]  P. Cleary,et al.  Perceived risks of heart disease and cancer among cigarette smokers. , 1999, JAMA.

[58]  R. Buehler,et al.  Frequency, Probability, and Prediction: Easy Solutions to Cognitive Illusions? , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.

[59]  J. Shepperd,et al.  Bracing for the Worst: Severity, Testing, and Feedback Timing as Moderators of the Optimistic Bias , 1998 .

[60]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Situated Optimism: Specific Outcome Expectancies and Self-Regulation , 1998 .

[61]  Y. Klar,et al.  No one in my group can be below the group's average: a robust positivity bias in favor of anonymous peers. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[62]  S. Byram,et al.  Cognitive and motivational factors influencing time prediction. , 1997 .

[63]  V. Clarke,et al.  Optimistic Bias in Beliefs about Smoking , 1997 .

[64]  M. Schoenbaum,et al.  Do smokers understand the mortality effects of smoking? Evidence from the Health and Retirement Survey. , 1997, American journal of public health.

[65]  W. Klein,et al.  Objective standards are not enough: affective, self-evaluative, and behavioral responses to social comparison information. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[66]  R. Buehler,et al.  The Role of Motivated Reasoning in Optimistic Time Predictions , 1997 .

[67]  D. Sarel,et al.  Nonunique invulnerability : Singular versus distributional probabilities and unrealistic optimism in comparative risk judgments , 1996 .

[68]  Alexander J. Rothman,et al.  Absolute and Relative Biases in Estimations of Personal Risk , 1996 .

[69]  J. Ouellette,et al.  Abandoning Unrealistic Optimism: Performance Estimates and the Temporal Proximity of Self-Relevant Feedback , 1996 .

[70]  Peter R. Harris,et al.  Sufficient grounds for optimism? The relationship between perceived controllability and optimistic bias , 1996 .

[71]  Wilma Otten,et al.  Context effects in the measurement of optimism in probability judgment , 1996 .

[72]  D. R. Lehman,et al.  Cultural variation in unrealistic optimism: Does the West feel more vulnerable than the East? , 1995 .

[73]  W. Klein,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: Resistance of Personal Risk Perceptions to Debiasing Interventions , 2002 .

[74]  V. Strecher,et al.  Changing inaccurate perceptions of health risk: results from a randomized trial. , 1995, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[75]  R. Buehler,et al.  Exploring the "planning fallacy": Why people underestimate their task completion times. , 1994 .

[76]  J. van der Pligt,et al.  Perceiving AIDS-related risks: accuracy as a function of differences in actual risk , 1994 .

[77]  J van der Pligt,et al.  Perceiving AIDS-related risk: accuracy as a function of differences in actual risk. , 1994, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[78]  R. O'Neil,et al.  Optimism in children's judgments of health and environmental risks. , 1994, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[79]  F P McKenna,et al.  Exploring the limits of optimism: the case of smokers' decision making. , 1993, British journal of psychology.

[80]  N D Weinstein,et al.  Correct and incorrect interpretations of correlations between risk perceptions and risk behaviors. , 1993, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[81]  Bram P. Buunk,et al.  SOCIAL-COMPARISON OF HEALTH RISKS - LOCUS OF CONTROL, THE PERSON-POSITIVITY BIAS, AND UNREALISTIC OPTIMISM , 1993 .

[82]  Frank P. McKenna,et al.  It won't happen to me: Unrealistic optimism or illusion of control? , 1993 .

[83]  Elaine Vaughan,et al.  Chronic exposure to an environmental hazard: risk perceptions and self-protective behavior. , 1993, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[84]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Optimism, coping, psychological distress, and high-risk sexual behavior among men at risk for acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS). , 1992, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[85]  J. Burger,et al.  Changes in and Generalization of Unrealistic Optimism Following Experiences with Stressful Events: Reactions to the 1989 California Earthquake , 1992 .

[86]  D. Dunning,et al.  Depression, realism, and the overconfidence effect : are the sadder wiser when predicting future actions and events ? , 1991 .

[87]  B. Whitley,et al.  Perceptions of Vulnerability to Pregnancy and the Use of Effective Contraception , 1991 .

[88]  F. Gibbons,et al.  Effects of reviewing risk-relevant behavior on perceived vulnerability among women marines. , 1991, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[89]  D. Dunning,et al.  Depression, realism, and the overconfidence effect: are the sadder wiser when predicting future actions and events? , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[90]  I. Rosenstock,et al.  The health belief model: Explaining health behavior through expectancies. , 1990 .

[91]  M. Gail,et al.  Projecting individualized probabilities of developing breast cancer for white females who are being examined annually. , 1989, Journal of the National Cancer Institute.

[92]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Illusion and well-being: a social psychological perspective on mental health. , 1988, Psychological bulletin.

[93]  Charles S. Carver,et al.  A Model of Behavioral Self-Regulation: Translating Intention into Action , 1988 .

[94]  L. Perloff,et al.  Self–other judgments and perceived vulnerability to victimization. , 1986 .

[95]  M. Becker,et al.  The Health Belief Model: A Decade Later , 1984, Health education quarterly.

[96]  R. W. Rogers,et al.  Protection motivation and self-efficacy: A revised theory of fear appeals and attitude change , 1983 .

[97]  Neil D. Weinstein,et al.  Egocentrism as a Source of Unrealistic Optimism , 1982 .

[98]  N. Weinstein Unrealistic optimism about future life events , 1980 .

[99]  P. Slovic,et al.  Dominance of accuracy information and neglect of base rates in probability estimation , 1976 .

[100]  R. W. Rogers,et al.  A Protection Motivation Theory of Fear Appeals and Attitude Change1. , 1975, The Journal of psychology.

[101]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.