Universals and Writing System Variations

The neural substrate for reading includes highly general subsystems, collectively the known as the “reading network.” To the extent that these subsystems are universal, they must somehow support the reading of a wide range of languages with a diverse set of written forms and mapping principles. In this chapter, we explore the highly contrastive cases of English and Chinese to examine how the neural basis of reading accommodates variability in the structure of languages. The notion of accommodation, in fact, is central to our analysis. We conclude that the reading network must accommodate variation in writing systems by organizing in a way that reflects the properties of each particular writing system. However, we also suggest that the prior state of the network—the organization of the network when there is a previously learned language—influences how the network adapts to those properties specific to a secondlanguage.Inparticular,tosomeextent,thepriornetworkmayassimilate the second language, using its first-language procedures to the extent possible. In elaborating this conclusion, we first review the orthographic structure and phonological mapping principles of Chinese and English, with some attention to how these two writing systems are learned by native speakers. We follow with a review of the comparative (across writing system) neuroscience research, and explore what the differences in brain regions used for Chinese and English might tell us about how these writing systems are processed. Finally, we review recent research that asks how the brain accommodates learning to read a second writing system.

[1]  Nobuhiko Akamatsu,et al.  The effects of first language orthographic features on word recognition processing in English as a second language , 1999 .

[2]  S E Shaywitz,et al.  Neurobiological studies of reading and reading disability. , 2001, Journal of communication disorders.

[3]  Ying Liu,et al.  Evidence for neural accommodation to a writing system following learning , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[4]  Jun-Young Chung,et al.  Neural mechanisms of Korean word reading: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study , 2005, Neuroscience Letters.

[5]  K. Koda,et al.  Alphabetic and nonalphabetic L1 effects in English word identification: a comparison of Korean and Chinese English L2 learners , 2003, Cognition.

[6]  R Todd Constable,et al.  The neurobiology of adaptive learning in reading: A contrast of different training conditions , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[7]  Julie A Fiez,et al.  Functional dissociations within the inferior parietal cortex in verbal working memory , 2004, NeuroImage.

[8]  I. Kanazawa,et al.  Different cortical activity in reading of Kanji words, Kana words and Kana nonwords. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[9]  J. Sergent The cerebral balance of power: confrontation or cooperation? , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  J. Jonides,et al.  Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. , 1999, Science.

[11]  Charles A. Perfetti,et al.  The time course of graphic, phonological, and semantic activation in Chinese character identification , 1998 .

[12]  Richard C. Anderson,et al.  Visual Chunking Skills of Hong Kong Children , 2005 .

[13]  Kyoung-Min Lee,et al.  Functional MRI comparison between reading ideographic and phonographic scripts of one language , 2004, Brain and Language.

[14]  A. Mechelli,et al.  Neuroimaging Studies of Word and Pseudoword Reading: Consistencies, Inconsistencies, and Limitations , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  M Coltheart,et al.  DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. , 2001, Psychological review.

[16]  P T Fox,et al.  Brain activation in the processing of Chinese characters and words: A functional MRI study , 2000, Human brain mapping.

[17]  C. Price The anatomy of language: contributions from functional neuroimaging , 2000, Journal of anatomy.

[18]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Subliminal Convergence of Kanji and Kana Words: Further Evidence for Functional Parcellation of the Posterior Temporal Cortex in Visual Word Perception , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  P. Fox,et al.  The Neural System Underlying Chinese Logograph Reading , 2001, NeuroImage.

[20]  S Lehéricy,et al.  The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[21]  Ying Liu,et al.  Orthography to Phonology and Meaning: Comparisons Across and within Writing Systems , 2005 .

[22]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Relations between the Neural Bases of Dynamic Auditory Processing and Phonological Processing: Evidence from fMRI , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[23]  Charles A. Perfetti,et al.  Threshold-style processing of Chinese characters for adult second-language learners , 2007, Memory & cognition.

[24]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Functional activation of the human frontal cortex during the performance of verbal working memory tasks. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[25]  Ying Liu,et al.  Alphabetic Readers Quickly Acquire Orthographic Structure in Learning to Read Chinese , 2003 .

[26]  L. Katz,et al.  Cerebral organization of component processes in reading. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[27]  A. Liberman,et al.  Functional disruption in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[28]  J. Marshall,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetries in global⧹local processing are modulated by perceptual salience , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  C. Perfetti,et al.  Phonological processes in reading Chinese characters. , 1991 .

[30]  P. Fox,et al.  Neuroanatomical correlates of phonological processing of Chinese characters and alphabetic words: A meta‐analysis , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[31]  Charles A. P Erfetti,et al.  Reading in two writing systems: Accommodation and assimilation of the brain's reading network , 2007, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[32]  Ying Liu,et al.  The lexical constituency model: some implications of research on Chinese for general theories of reading. , 2005, Psychological review.

[33]  Stephen Christman,et al.  Visual hemispheric asymmetries depend on which spatial frequencies are task relevant , 1992, Brain and Cognition.

[34]  Ying Liu,et al.  Visual Analysis and Lexical Access of Chinese Characters by Chinese as Second Language Readers , 2006 .

[35]  M. Chee,et al.  Mandarin and English Single Word Processing Studied with Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  W. Schneider,et al.  Cross‐cultural effect on the brain revisited: Universal structures plus writing system variation , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[37]  C. Perfetti,et al.  VERY EARLY PHONOLOGICAL ACTIVATION IN CHINESE READING , 1995 .

[38]  J. Fiez,et al.  Assimilation and accommodation patterns in ventral occipitotemporal cortex in learning a second writing system , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[39]  Paul W. B. Atkins,et al.  Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. , 1993 .

[40]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  An area specialized for spatial working memory in human frontal cortex. , 1998, Science.

[41]  A Pollatsek,et al.  The activation of phonology during silent Chinese word reading. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[42]  Ignatius G. Mattinglyt Morphological Structure and Segmental Awareness , 1987 .

[43]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[44]  J. Ziegler,et al.  Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[45]  D. Delis,et al.  Hemispheric specialization of memory for visual hierarchical stimuli , 1986, Neuropsychologia.

[46]  F. Chua Phonological recoding in Chinese logograph recognition. , 1999 .

[47]  Kit-Tai Hau,et al.  Learning to Read and Spell English Words by Chinese Students , 2005 .

[48]  Yanchao Bi,et al.  Reading does not depend on writing, even in Chinese , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[49]  Daisy L. Hung,et al.  Chapter 6 Automatic Activation of Linguistic Information in Chinese Character Recognition , 1992 .

[50]  K. Amunts,et al.  Localized morphological brain differences between English-speaking Caucasians and Chinese-speaking Asians: new evidence of anatomical plasticity , 2003, Neuroreport.

[51]  Ying Liu,et al.  ERP evidence for the time course of graphic, phonological, and semantic information in Chinese meaning and pronunciation decisions. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[52]  M. Kutas,et al.  Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. , 1980, Science.

[53]  J. Xiong,et al.  Neural systems of second language reading are shaped by native language , 2003 .

[54]  Yukihiko Fujii,et al.  Brain strategies for reading in the second language are determined by the first language , 2001, Neuroscience Research.

[55]  Joel R. Meyer,et al.  Brain–behavior correlation in children depends on the neurocognitive network , 2004, Human brain mapping.

[56]  L. Tan,et al.  Phonological Activation in Visual Identification of Chinese Two-Character Words , 1999 .

[57]  C. Perfetti,et al.  Reading in English and Chinese: Evidence for a "universal" phonological principle. , 1992 .

[58]  Karen M. Jones,et al.  Neural Changes following Remediation in Adult Developmental Dyslexia , 2004, Neuron.

[59]  L. Tan,et al.  Visual Chinese Character Recognition: Does Phonological Information Mediate Access to Meaning? , 1997 .

[60]  C. Perfetti,et al.  Phonemic activation during the first 40 ms of word identification: Evidence from backward masking and priming , 1991 .

[61]  Michael D. Rugg,et al.  Event-related potentials in phonological matching tasks , 1984, Brain and Language.

[62]  L. Tan,et al.  Biological abnormality of impaired reading is constrained by culture , 2004, Nature.

[63]  James R. Booth,et al.  The Development of Specialized Brain Systems in Reading and Oral-Language , 2001, Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence.

[64]  F. Kitterle,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetry in the processing of absolute versus relative spatial frequency , 1991, Brain and Cognition.

[65]  Esther Geva,et al.  Spelling performance of Chinese children using English as a second language: Lexical and visual–orthographic processes , 2003, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[66]  C. Perfetti,et al.  The tongue-twister effect in reading Chinese. , 1993 .

[67]  James R. Booth,et al.  Cultural Constraints on Brain Development: Evidence from a Developmental Study of Visual Word Processing in Mandarin Chinese , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[68]  S. Petersen,et al.  Neuroimaging studies of word reading. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[69]  Stefano F. Cappa,et al.  The bilingual brain as revealed by functional neuroimaging , 2001, The Bilingualism Reader.

[70]  Guinevere F. Eden,et al.  Meta-Analysis of the Functional Neuroanatomy of Single-Word Reading: Method and Validation , 2002, NeuroImage.

[71]  M. Chee,et al.  Overlap and Dissociation of Semantic Processing of Chinese Characters, English Words, and Pictures: Evidence from fMRI , 2000, NeuroImage.

[72]  Ying Liu,et al.  The time course of brain activity in reading English and Chinese: An ERP study of Chinese bilinguals , 2003, Human brain mapping.

[73]  Joel R. Meyer,et al.  Relation between brain activation and lexical performance , 2003, Human brain mapping.