Valuing what happens: a biogenic approach to valence and (potentially) affect

Valence is half of the pair of properties that constitute core affect, the foundation of emotion. But what is valence, and where is it found in the natural world? Currently, this question cannot be answered. The idea that emotion is the body's way of driving the organism to secure its survival, thriving and reproduction runs like a leitmotif from the pathfinding work of Antonio Damasio through four book-length neuroscientific accounts of emotion recently published by the field's leading practitioners. Yet while Damasio concluded 20 years ago that the homeostasis–affect linkage is rooted in unicellular life, no agreement exists about whether even non-human animals with brains experience emotions. Simple neural animals—those less brainy than bees, fruit flies and other charismatic invertebrates—are not even on the radar of contemporary affective research, to say nothing of aneural organisms. This near-sightedness has effectively denied the most productive method available for getting a grip on highly complex biological processes to a scientific domain whose importance for understanding biological decision-making cannot be underestimated. Valence arguably is the fulcrum around which the dance of life revolves. Without the ability to discriminate advantage from harm, life very quickly comes to an end. In this paper, we review the concept of valence, where it came from, the work it does in current leading theories of emotion, and some of the odd features revealed via experiment. We present a biologically grounded framework for investigating valence in any organism and sketch a preliminary pathway to a computational model. This article is part of the theme issue ‘Basal cognition: conceptual tools and the view from the single cell’.

[1]  A. Binet The psychic life of micro-organisms : a study in experimental psychology / by Alfred Binet. , 1897 .

[2]  R. Yerkes Contributions to the Study of the Behavior of Lower Organisms , 1904 .

[3]  M. Abercrombie,et al.  A dictionary of biology , 1951 .

[4]  M. Meehan General System Theory: Foundations, Development, Applications , 1969 .

[5]  J. Gray,et al.  The psychology of fear and stress , 1971 .

[6]  Howard Hunt Pattee,et al.  Hierarchy Theory: The Challenge of Complex Systems , 1973 .

[7]  J. Adler,et al.  "Decision"-Making in Bacteria: Chemotactic Response of Escherichia coli to Conflicting Stimuli , 1974, Science.

[8]  W. Elsasser The chief abstractions of biology , 1975 .

[9]  J. D. E. Koshland Bacterial chemotaxis as a model behavioral system , 1980 .

[10]  A. Tversky,et al.  Variants of uncertainty , 1982, Cognition.

[11]  R. Bornstein Exposure and affect: Overview and meta-analysis of research, 1968–1987. , 1989 .

[12]  R. Rosen Life Itself: A Comprehensive Inquiry Into the Nature, Origin, and Fabrication of Life , 1991 .

[13]  R. Reisenzein A structuralist reconstruction of Wundt's three-dimensional theory of emotion. , 1992 .

[14]  Sean A. Spence,et al.  Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain , 1995 .

[15]  R. Joynt Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain , 1995 .

[16]  S. Kauffman At Home in the Universe: The Search for the Laws of Self-Organization and Complexity , 1995 .

[17]  Andrew W. Moore,et al.  Reinforcement Learning: A Survey , 1996, J. Artif. Intell. Res..

[18]  Peter Dayan,et al.  A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward , 1997, Science.

[19]  A. Moreno,et al.  Cognition and Life: The Autonomy of Cognition , 1997, Brain and Cognition.

[20]  K. Popper All life is problem solving , 1997 .

[21]  I. Prigogine,et al.  The end of certainty : time, chaos, and the new laws of nature , 1997 .

[22]  Peter A. Corning,et al.  Thermodynamics, information and life revisited, Part II: ‘Thermoeconomics’ and ‘Control information’ , 1998 .

[23]  S. J. Kline,et al.  Thermodynamics, information and life revisited, Part I: ‘To be or entropy’ , 1998 .

[24]  J. Russell,et al.  Independence and bipolarity in the structure of current affect. , 1998 .

[25]  Claus Emmeche,et al.  Defining life as a semiotic phenomenon , 1998, Cybern. Hum. Knowing.

[26]  A. Damasio The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness , 1999 .

[27]  P. Postma,et al.  Global regulatory networks and signal transduction pathways. , 1999 .

[28]  W. Christensen,et al.  Autonomy and the emergence of intelligence: Organised interactive construction , 2000 .

[29]  K. Vohs,et al.  Case Western Reserve University , 1990 .

[30]  K. Hellingwerf,et al.  Autoamplification of a Two-Component Regulatory System Results in “Learning” Behavior , 2001, Journal of bacteriology.

[31]  R. Zajonc Mere Exposure: A Gateway to the Subliminal , 2001 .

[32]  Karam B. Singh,et al.  Transcription factors in plant defense and stress responses. , 2002, Current opinion in plant biology.

[33]  P. Griffiths What is Innateness , 2002 .

[34]  M. Cabanac What is emotion? , 2002, Behavioural Processes.

[35]  J. Russell Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. , 2003, Psychological review.

[36]  Heather M. Claypool,et al.  Positivity Can Cue Familiarity , 2004, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[37]  R. Samuels Innateness in cognitive science , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[38]  E. Jablonka,et al.  Evolution in Four Dimensions: Genetic, Epigenetic, Behavioral, and Symbolic Variation in the History of Life , 2005 .

[39]  R. Dolan,et al.  Unconscious fear influences emotional awareness of faces and voices. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[40]  Pamela Lyon,et al.  The biogenic approach to cognition , 2006, Cognitive Processing.

[41]  Richard S. Sutton,et al.  Reinforcement Learning: An Introduction , 1998, IEEE Trans. Neural Networks.

[42]  K. Roelofs,et al.  The effects of stress-induced cortisol responses on approach–avoidance behavior , 2005, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[43]  R. H. Phaf,et al.  Affective modulation of recognition bias. , 2005, Emotion.

[44]  M. Mendl,et al.  Measuring emotional processes in animals: the utility of a cognitive approach , 2005, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[45]  L. F. Barrett Valence is a basic building block of emotional life , 2006 .

[46]  P. Lyon The agent in the organism : towards a biogenic theory of cognition , 2006 .

[47]  L. F. Barrett Are Emotions Natural Kinds? , 2006, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[48]  R. Melano,et al.  The Helicobacter pylori Chemotaxis Receptor TlpB (HP0103) Is Required for pH Taxis and for Colonization of the Gastric Mucosa , 2006, Journal of bacteriology.

[49]  M. Mameli,et al.  The innate and the acquired: useful clusters or a residual distinction from folk biology? , 2007, Developmental psychobiology.

[50]  S. Stanford The Open Field Test: reinventing the wheel , 2007, Journal of psychopharmacology.

[51]  Saeed Tavazoie,et al.  Predictive Behavior Within Microbial Genetic Networks , 2008, Science.

[52]  J. Keasling,et al.  Memory in Microbes: Quantifying History-Dependent Behavior in a Bacterium , 2008, PloS one.

[53]  K. Lewin,et al.  A Dynamic Theory of Personality - Selected Papers , 2008 .

[54]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  DEM: A variational treatment of dynamic systems , 2008, NeuroImage.

[55]  W. Gerstner,et al.  Stress, genotype and norepinephrine in the prediction of mouse behavior using reinforcement learning , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[56]  E. Ben-Jacob Learning from Bacteria about Natural Information Processing , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[57]  Karl J. Friston The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[58]  D. Martinez,et al.  FoxO and Stress Responses in the Cnidarian Hydra vulgaris , 2010, PloS one.

[59]  Richard A. Watson,et al.  Associative Memory in Gene Regulation Networks , 2010, ALIFE.

[60]  B. Taborsky,et al.  Environmental Change Enhances Cognitive Abilities in Fish , 2010, PLoS biology.

[61]  M. Butts,et al.  Are Bad Experiences Stronger than Good Ones in Mentoring Relationships? Evidence from the Protege and Mentor Perspective. , 2010 .

[62]  S. Laughlin Energy, Information, and the Work of the Brain , 2011 .

[63]  Tomohiro Shirakawa,et al.  An associative learning experiment using the plasmodium of Physarum polycephalum , 2011, Nano Commun. Networks.

[64]  G. Storz,et al.  Bacterial stress responses. , 2011 .

[65]  M. Mameli,et al.  An evaluation of the concept of innateness , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[66]  N. Shea Genetic Representation Explains the Cluster of Innateness‐Related Properties , 2012 .

[67]  Joseph E LeDoux Rethinking the Emotional Brain , 2012, Neuron.

[68]  H. Pattee How Does a Molecule Become a Message , 2012 .

[69]  Phil Husbands,et al.  Evolution of Associative Learning in Chemical Networks , 2012, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[70]  R. Parthasarathy,et al.  Structure and proposed mechanism for the pH-sensing Helicobacter pylori chemoreceptor TlpB. , 2012, Structure.

[71]  V. Sourjik,et al.  Opposite responses by different chemoreceptors set a tunable preference point in Escherichia coli pH taxis , 2012, Molecular microbiology.

[72]  Naoaki Kawakami The Implicit Influence of a Negative Mood on the Subliminal Mere Exposure Effect , 2012, Perceptual and motor skills.

[73]  Jeffrey V. Nickerson,et al.  Anticipatory systems: philosophical, mathematical, and methodological foundations , 2012, Int. J. Gen. Syst..

[74]  P. Goel,et al.  Learning Theories Reveal Loss of Pancreatic Electrical Connectivity in Diabetes as an Adaptive Response , 2013, PloS one.

[75]  Evava S. Pietri,et al.  Weighting positive versus negative: the fundamental nature of valence asymmetry. , 2013, Journal of personality.

[76]  Clint J. Perry,et al.  Neural mechanisms of reward in insects. , 2013, Annual review of entomology.

[77]  Karl J. Friston Life as we know it , 2013, Journal of The Royal Society Interface.

[78]  A. Craig How Do You Feel?: An Interoceptive Moment with Your Neurobiological Self , 2014 .

[79]  David J. Anderson,et al.  A Framework for Studying Emotions across Species , 2014, Cell.

[80]  William D. Penny,et al.  A Dynamic Bayesian Model of Homeostatic Control , 2014, ICAIS.

[81]  Mehdi Keramati,et al.  Homeostatic reinforcement learning for integrating reward collection and physiological stability , 2014, eLife.

[82]  Gürol M. Süel,et al.  Metabolic codependence gives rise to collective oscillations within biofilms , 2015, Nature.

[83]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Knowing one's place: a free-energy approach to pattern regulation , 2015, Journal of The Royal Society Interface.

[84]  B. McEwen,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews Stress Habituation, Body Shape and Cardiovascular Mortality , 2022 .

[85]  Gürol M. Süel,et al.  Ion channels enable electrical communication in bacterial communities , 2015, Nature.

[86]  H. Vlamakis,et al.  From Cell Differentiation to Cell Collectives: Bacillus subtilis Uses Division of Labor to Migrate , 2015, PLoS biology.

[87]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Active inference and epistemic value , 2015, Cognitive neuroscience.

[88]  Peter Sterling,et al.  Principles of Neural Design , 2015 .

[89]  A. Dussutour,et al.  Habituation in non-neural organisms: evidence from slime moulds , 2016, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[90]  C. Unkelbach,et al.  A general valence asymmetry in similarity: Good is more alike than bad. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[91]  R. Dolan,et al.  Computations of uncertainty mediate acute stress responses in humans , 2016, Nature Communications.

[92]  B. Sonntag,et al.  Stress and Protists: No life without stress. , 2016, European journal of protistology.

[93]  Maria Fors Brandebo,et al.  Leadership: is bad stronger than good? , 2016 .

[94]  Miguel A. Vadillo,et al.  Underpowered samples, false negatives, and unconscious learning , 2015, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[95]  Ling Ren,et al.  Is bad stronger than good? The impact of police-citizen encounters on public satisfaction with police , 2016 .

[96]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Uncertainty and stress: Why it causes diseases and how it is mastered by the brain , 2017, Progress in Neurobiology.

[97]  C. Unkelbach,et al.  Why Good Is More Alike Than Bad: Processing Implications , 2017, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[98]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Stress enhances model-free reinforcement learning only after negative outcome , 2017, PloS one.

[99]  Clint J. Perry,et al.  Studying emotion in invertebrates: what has been done, what can be measured and what they can provide , 2017, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[100]  Melanie M. van der Ploeg,et al.  Peripheral physiological responses to subliminally presented negative affective stimuli: A systematic review , 2017, Biological Psychology.

[101]  Jack L. Vevea,et al.  A Re-Examination of the Mere Exposure Effect: The Influence of Repeated Exposure on Recognition, Familiarity, and Liking , 2017, Psychological bulletin.

[102]  L. F. Barrett How Emotions Are Made: The Secret Life of the Brain , 2017 .

[103]  L. F. Barrett The theory of constructed emotion: an active inference account of interoception and categorization , 2016, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[104]  Paul A. Wiggins,et al.  Noise in a phosphorelay drives stochastic entry into sporulation in Bacillus subtilis , 2017, The EMBO journal.

[105]  I. Mandic-Mulec,et al.  Social behaviours by Bacillus subtilis: quorum sensing, kin discrimination and beyond , 2018, Molecular microbiology.

[106]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Surviving threats: neural circuit and computational implications of a new taxonomy of defensive behaviour , 2018, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[107]  J. de Houwer,et al.  Mere Exposure Effects on Implicit Stimulus Evaluation: The Moderating Role of Evaluation Task, Number of Stimulus Presentations, and Memory for Presentation Frequency , 2018, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[108]  K. Berridge Evolving Concepts of Emotion and Motivation , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[109]  David J. Anderson,et al.  The Neuroscience of Emotion: A New Synthesis , 2018 .

[110]  Stefania Paolini,et al.  Bad Is Stronger Than Good for Stigmatized, but Not Admired Outgroups: Meta-Analytical Tests of Intergroup Valence Asymmetry in Individual-to-Group Generalization Experiments , 2019, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[111]  M. Manson pH Sensing in Bacillus subtilis: a New Path to a Common Goal , 2019, Journal of bacteriology.

[112]  A. Becker,et al.  Phenotypic Heterogeneity in Bacterial Quorum Sensing Systems. , 2019, Journal of molecular biology.

[113]  Yun Zhang,et al.  Molecular and cellular modulators for multisensory integration in C. elegans , 2019, PLoS genetics.

[114]  C. V. Rao,et al.  The Mechanism of Bidirectional pH Taxis in Bacillus subtilis , 2019, Journal of bacteriology.

[115]  Hannah Jeckel,et al.  Learning the space-time phase diagram of bacterial swarm expansion , 2019, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[116]  P. Griffiths,et al.  Scientists’ Concepts of Innateness: Evolution or Attraction? , 2019, Advances in Experimental Philosophy of Science.

[117]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Morphogenesis as Bayesian inference: A variational approach to pattern formation and control in complex biological systems. , 2020, Physics of life reviews.

[118]  F. Yoshida,et al.  Subliminal versus supraliminal mere exposure effects: Comparing explicit and implicit attitudes. , 2019, Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice.

[119]  Gürol M. Süel,et al.  Metabolic basis of brain-like electrical signalling in bacterial communities , 2019, bioRxiv.

[120]  R. Losick Bacillus subtilis: a bacterium for all seasons , 2020, Current Biology.

[121]  Gürol M. Süel,et al.  Encoding Membrane-Potential-Based Memory within a Microbial Community. , 2020, Cell systems.

[122]  F. Keijzer,et al.  Reframing cognition: getting down to biological basics , 2021, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B.