Dissociable Contributions of Prefrontal and Parietal Cortices to Response Selection

The ability to select between possible responses to a given situation is central to human cognition. The goal of this study was to distinguish between brain areas representing candidate responses and areas selecting between competing response alternatives. Event-related fMRI data were acquired while 10 healthy adults performed a task used to examine response competition: the Eriksen flanker task. Left parietal cortex was activated by either of two manipulations that increased the need to maintain a representation of possible responses. In contrast, lateral prefrontal and rostral anterior cingulate cortices were specifically engaged by the need to select among competing response alternatives. These findings support the idea that parietal cortex is involved in activating possible responses on the basis of learned stimulus-response associations, and that prefrontal cortex is recruited when there is a need to select between competing responses.

[1]  G. Glover,et al.  Self‐navigated spiral fMRI: Interleaved versus single‐shot , 1998, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[2]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Neural Activation During Response Competition , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  A. Holmes Generalisability, random effects and population inference (abstract) , 1998 .

[4]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A PET study of visuospatial attention , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[5]  D. Pandya,et al.  Projections to the frontal cortex from the posterior parietal region in the rhesus monkey , 1984, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[6]  J. Desmond,et al.  Prefrontal regions involved in keeping information in and out of mind. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Top-down signal from prefrontal cortex in executive control of memory retrieval , 1999, Nature.

[8]  M. Hallett,et al.  Cerebral structures participating in motor preparation in humans: a positron emission tomography study. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Common inhibitory mechanism in human inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by event-related functional MRI. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[10]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Dissociation of Storage and Rehearsal in Verbal Working Memory: Evidence From Positron Emission Tomography , 1996 .

[11]  R. Ivry,et al.  Response channel activation and the lateral prefrontal cortex , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  A. Sirigu,et al.  The Mental Representation of Hand Movements After Parietal Cortex Damage , 1996, Science.

[13]  R. Passingham,et al.  Specialisation within the prefrontal cortex: the ventral prefrontal cortex and associative learning , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Cortex, Conflict Monitoring, and Levels of Processing , 2001, NeuroImage.

[15]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 2004 .

[16]  Matthew Flatt,et al.  PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers , 1993 .

[17]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  D. Stuss,et al.  The Frontal Lobes , 1986 .

[19]  A. Luria Higher Cortical Functions in Man , 1980, Springer US.

[20]  R. Andersen,et al.  Intention-related activity in the posterior parietal cortex: a review , 2000, Vision Research.

[21]  T. Robbins,et al.  Planning and spatial working memory following frontal lobe lesions in man , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  S. Kornblum,et al.  Isolation of Specific Interference Processing in the Stroop Task: PET Activation Studies , 1997, NeuroImage.

[23]  M. Petrides Deficits on conditional associative-learning tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man , 1985, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Cognition and control in schizophrenia: a computational model of dopamine and prefrontal function , 1999, Biological Psychiatry.

[25]  Asher Cohen,et al.  Perceptual Dimensional Constraints in Response Selection Processes , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[26]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature.

[27]  R. Passingham,et al.  Learning Arbitrary Visuomotor Associations: Temporal Dynamic of Brain Activity , 2001, NeuroImage.

[28]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Immature Frontal Lobe Contributions to Cognitive Control in Children Evidence from fMRI , 2002, Neuron.

[29]  T. Bussey,et al.  Role of prefrontal cortex in a network for arbitrary visuomotor mapping , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  John D. E. Gabrieli,et al.  Material-dependent and material-independent selection processes in the frontal and parietal lobes: an event-related fMRI investigation of response competition , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  The Role of Parietal Cortex in Verbal Working Memory , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  No‐go dominant brain activity in human inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging , 1998, The European journal of neuroscience.

[33]  L. Kempe Handbook of Physiology. Section I. The Nervous System , 1982 .

[34]  M. Hallett,et al.  Frontal and parietal networks for conditional motor learning: a positron emission tomography study. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  M. Rushworth,et al.  Attention Systems and the Organization of the Human Parietal Cortex , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  G R Grice,et al.  Combination rule for redundant information in reaction time tasks with divided attention , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[37]  M. Farah,et al.  Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: a reevaluation. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[38]  E. Miller,et al.  The Prefrontal Cortex Complex Neural Properties for Complex Behavior , 1999, Neuron.

[39]  B. Milner,et al.  Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man , 1982, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[41]  M. Mintun,et al.  Positron emission tomography study of voluntary saccadic eye movements and spatial working memory. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  The neural correlates of the verbal component of working memory , 1993, Nature.

[43]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Inactivation of parietal and prefrontal cortex reveals interdependence of neural activity during memory-guided saccades. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  K. Heilman,et al.  Two forms of ideomotor apraxia , 1982, Neurology.

[45]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[46]  J. Cohen,et al.  Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. , 2000, Science.

[47]  J. Desmond,et al.  Dissociation of Frontal and Cerebellar Activity in a Cognitive Task: Evidence for a Distinction between Selection and Search , 1998, NeuroImage.

[48]  P. Strick,et al.  Imaging the premotor areas , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[49]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Deciding not to GO: neuronal correlates of response selection in a GO/NOGO task in primate premotor and parietal cortex. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.

[50]  B. Milner Effects of Different Brain Lesions on Card Sorting: The Role of the Frontal Lobes , 1963 .

[51]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  A Developmental Functional MRI Study of Prefrontal Activation during Performance of a Go-No-Go Task , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[52]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Coactivation of prefrontal cortex and inferior parietal cortex in working memory tasks revealed by 2DG functional mapping in the rhesus monkey , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[53]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[54]  P S Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Callosal and intrahemispheric connectivity of the prefrontal association cortex in rhesus monkey: Relation between intraparietal and principal sulcal cortex , 1984, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[55]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Perception of self-generated movement following left parietal lesion. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[56]  J D Gabrieli,et al.  A resource model of the neural basis of executive working memory. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[57]  R. O’Reilly,et al.  A computational approach to prefrontal cortex, cognitive control and schizophrenia: recent developments and current challenges. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[58]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Mapping motor representations with positron emission tomography , 1994, Nature.

[59]  B. Milner,et al.  Performance of complex arm and facial movements after focal brain lesions , 1981, Neuropsychologia.

[60]  G. Micheletti The Prefrontal Cortex. Anatomy, Physiology and Neuropsychology of the Frontal Lobe, Fuster J.M.. Raven Press, New York (1989) , 1989 .

[61]  N. Geschwind The apraxias: neural mechanisms of disorders of learned movement. , 1975, American scientist.

[62]  J. Jonides,et al.  Inhibition in verbal working memory revealed by brain activation. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[63]  E. Donchin,et al.  Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[64]  P. Skudlarski,et al.  An event-related functional MRI study of the stroop color word interference task. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[65]  J. Fuster,et al.  Effects of cooling parietal cortex on prefrontal units in delay tasks , 1989, Brain Research.

[66]  E. Stein,et al.  Right hemispheric dominance of inhibitory control: an event-related functional MRI study. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[67]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[68]  E. Crone,et al.  Dissociation of response conflict, attentional selection, and expectancy with functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[69]  R. Passingham,et al.  The prefrontal cortex: response selection or maintenance within working memory? , 2000, 5th IEEE EMBS International Summer School on Biomedical Imaging, 2002..

[70]  D. Brooks,et al.  Motor sequence learning: a study with positron emission tomography , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[71]  M. Raichle,et al.  The anterior cingulate cortex mediates processing selection in the Stroop attentional conflict paradigm. , 1990, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[72]  R. Passingham,et al.  The Attentional Role of the Left Parietal Cortex: The Distinct Lateralization and Localization of Motor Attention in the Human Brain , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[73]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Localization of grasp representations in humans by positron emission tomography , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[74]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Attention and stimulus characteristics determine the locus of motor-sequence encoding. A PET study. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[75]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[76]  R. Passingham,et al.  Ventral Prefrontal Cortex Is Not Essential for Working Memory , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.