Dissociative effects of normative feedback on motor automaticity and motor accuracy in learning an arm movement sequence.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Timothy D. Lee,et al. Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral Emphasis , 1982 .
[2] Alan M. Wing,et al. 7 – Doing Two Things at Once: Process Limitations and Interactions , 1984 .
[3] Jutta Heckhausen,et al. Motivation and action , 1991 .
[4] A. Kluger,et al. The effects of feedback interventions on performance: A historical review, a meta-analysis, and a preliminary feedback intervention theory. , 1996 .
[5] G. Mavromatis,et al. Goal Setting and Feedback for the Development of Instructional Strategies , 1997, Perceptual and motor skills.
[6] Attention! , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[7] K. Doya,et al. Parallel neural networks for learning sequential procedures , 1999, Trends in Neurosciences.
[8] TWO PROCEDURES, ONE MECHANISM? RECENT FINDINGS ON THE AUTOMATION OF VOLUNTARY MOVEMENTS , 2000 .
[9] D. Ilgen,et al. Bearing Bad News: Reactions to Negative Performance Feedback , 2000 .
[10] Justin T. Maxwell,et al. The implicit benefit of learning without errors , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[11] Kae Nakamura,et al. Central mechanisms of motor skill learning , 2002, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[12] Clay B. Holroyd,et al. The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.
[13] E. A. Locke,et al. Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task motivation. A 35-year odyssey. , 2002, The American psychologist.
[14] Nicole Wenderoth,et al. Changes in Brain Activation during the Acquisition of a Multifrequency Bimanual Coordination Task: From the Cognitive Stage to Advanced Levels of Automaticity , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[15] J. C. Johnston,et al. How does practice reduce dual-task interference: Integration, automatization, or just stage-shortening? , 2006, Psychological research.
[16] M. Botvinick. Conflict monitoring and decision making: Reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[17] J. Maxwell,et al. Passing thoughts on the evolutionary stability of implicit motor behaviour: Performance retention under physiological fatigue , 2007, Consciousness and Cognition.
[18] O. Hikosaka,et al. Perceptual Learning, Motor Learning and Automaticity Switching from Automatic to Controlled Behavior: Cortico-basal Ganglia Mechanisms , 2022 .
[19] Florian Wagner,et al. Dual-task Practice of Temporally Structured Movement Sequences Augments Integrated Task Processing, but not Automatization , 2010 .
[20] Gabriele Wulf,et al. Normative Feedback Effects on Learning a Timing Task , 2010, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.
[21] N. Hagemann,et al. The dimensionality of the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory: An analysis with models of the item response theory , 2010, Laterality.
[22] J. Maxwell,et al. Probing the allocation of attention in implicit (motor) learning , 2010, Journal of sports sciences.
[23] G. Wulf,et al. Social-comparative feedback affects motor skill learning. , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[24] P. Glimcher. Understanding dopamine and reinforcement learning: The dopamine reward prediction error hypothesis , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[25] Jae T Patterson,et al. Knowing the good from the bad: does being aware of KR content matter? , 2012, Human movement science.
[26] Gabriele Wulf,et al. Positive social-comparative feedback enhances motor learning in children , 2012 .
[27] Rouwen Cañal-Bruland,et al. Error Reduction during Practice: A Novel Method for Learning to Kick Free-Kicks in Soccer , 2012 .
[28] Walter Schneider,et al. The Brain’s Learning and Control Architecture , 2012 .
[29] Brett W Fling,et al. Neurocognitive mechanisms of error-based motor learning. , 2013, Advances in experimental medicine and biology.
[30] J. Poolton,et al. Reducing errors benefits the field‐based learning of a fundamental movement skill in children , 2013, Scandinavian journal of medicine & science in sports.
[31] N. Hodges,et al. Manipulating target size influences perceptions of success when learning a dart-throwing skill but does not impact retention , 2015, Front. Psychol..
[32] Michael J Carter,et al. Judgments of learning are significantly higher following feedback on relatively good versus relatively poor trials despite no actual learning differences. , 2016, Human movement science.
[33] G. Wulf,et al. Optimizing performance through intrinsic motivation and attention for learning: The OPTIMAL theory of motor learning , 2016, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
[34] D. Krause,et al. Effects of bandwidth feedback on the automatization of an arm movement sequence. , 2016, Human movement science.
[35] D. Feltz,et al. Self‐doubt: Uncertainty as a motivating factor on effort in an exercise endurance task , 2017 .
[36] N. Hodges,et al. Balancing Our Perceptions of the Efficacy of Success-Based Feedback Manipulations on Motor Learning , 2018, Journal of motor behavior.
[37] D. Krause,et al. Error Feedback Frequency Affects Automaticity But Not Accuracy and Consistency After Extensive Motor Skill Practice , 2018, Journal of motor behavior.
[38] S. T. Klapp,et al. The bottleneck of the psychological refractory period effect involves timing of response initiation rather than response selection , 2018, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.