Advert saliency distracts children's visual attention during task-oriented internet use

The general research question of the present study was to assess the impact of visually salient online adverts on children's task-oriented internet use. In order to answer this question, an experimental study was constructed in which 9- and 12-year-old Swedish children were asked to solve a number of tasks while interacting with a mockup website. In each trial, web adverts in several saliency conditions were presented. By both measuring children's task accuracy, as well as the visual processing involved in solving these tasks, this study allows us to infer how two types of visual saliency affect children's attentional behavior, and whether such behavioral effects also impacts their task performance. Analyses show that low-level visual features and task relevance in online adverts have different effects on performance measures and process measures respectively. Whereas task performance is stable with regard to several advert saliency conditions, a marked effect is seen on children's gaze behavior. On the other hand, task performance is shown to be more sensitive to individual differences such as age, gender and level of gaze control. The results provide evidence about cognitive and behavioral distraction effects in children's task-oriented internet use caused by visual saliency in online adverts. The experiment suggests that children to some extent are able to compensate for behavioral effects caused by distracting visual stimuli when solving prospective memory tasks. Suggestions are given for further research into the interdiciplinary area between media research and cognitive science.

[1]  N. Lavie Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  L H HARDY,et al.  Tests for detection and analysis of color blindness; an evaluation of the Ishihara test. , 1945, Archives of ophthalmology.

[3]  Peter König,et al.  Influence of disparity on fixation and saccades in free viewing of natural scenes. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[4]  C. Klein,et al.  Development of prosaccade and antisaccade task performance in participants aged 6 to 26 years. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[5]  Asha Iyer,et al.  Components of bottom-up gaze allocation in natural images , 2005, Vision Research.

[6]  J. Henderson Human gaze control during real-world scene perception , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  K. Richard Ridderinkhof,et al.  Age-related changes in antisaccade task performance: Inhibitory control or working-memory engagement? , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[8]  Linden J. Ball,et al.  An Eye Movement Analysis of Web Page Usability , 2002 .

[9]  D. Kerzel,et al.  Influence of target and distractor contrast on the remote distractor effect , 2008, Vision Research.

[10]  Marcus Nyström,et al.  Fit-for-duty test for estimation of drivers' sleepiness level : Eye movements improve the sleep/wake predictor , 2013 .

[11]  R. Walker,et al.  A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[12]  Children’s Views and Practices Regarding Online Advertising An Interview Study with Swedish Nine-Year-Olds , 2013 .

[13]  A. L. Yarbus,et al.  Eye Movements and Vision , 1967, Springer US.

[14]  T. Gog,et al.  In the eyes of the beholder: How experts and novices interpret dynamic stimuli , 2010 .

[15]  T. Foulsham,et al.  What can saliency models predict about eye movements? Spatial and sequential aspects of fixations during encoding and recognition. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[16]  C. Koch,et al.  A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention , 2000, Vision Research.

[17]  S. Hutton,et al.  The antisaccade task as a research tool in psychopathology: a critical review. , 2006, Psychophysiology.

[18]  Iain D Gilchrist,et al.  Oculomotor capture by transient events: a comparison of abrupt onsets, offsets, motion, and flicker. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[19]  I. Hooge,et al.  Inhibition of return is not a foraging facilitator in saccadic search and free viewing , 2005, Vision Research.

[20]  C. Snider,et al.  The Eyes , 1877, Canadian Medical Association Journal.

[21]  D. Munoz,et al.  Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[22]  Muriel Walshe,et al.  Eye tracking in schizophrenia: Does the antisaccade task measure anything that the smooth pursuit task does not? , 2005, Psychiatry Research.

[23]  A. L. I︠A︡rbus Eye Movements and Vision , 1967 .

[24]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention , 2003, Vision Research.

[25]  Nils Holmberg,et al.  The use of eye-tracking and retrospective interviews to study teenagers’ exposure to online advertising , 2012 .

[26]  G. Zelinsky Using Eye Saccades to Assess the Selectivity of Search Movements , 1996, Vision Research.

[27]  Eva A. van Reijmersdal,et al.  Introducing the PCMC Model: An Investigative Framework for Young People's Processing of Commercialized Media Content , 2010 .

[28]  N. Cassavaugh,et al.  Development of attentional and oculomotor control. , 2005, Developmental psychology.

[29]  George L. Malcolm,et al.  Combining top-down processes to guide eye movements during real-world scene search. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[30]  Sonia Livingstone,et al.  Zero to eight: young children and their internet use , 2013 .

[31]  Christopher Kennard,et al.  An internationally standardised antisaccade protocol , 2013, Vision Research.

[32]  Kenneth Holmqvist,et al.  Eye tracking: a comprehensive guide to methods and measures , 2011 .

[33]  D. Ballard,et al.  Eye guidance in natural vision: reinterpreting salience. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[34]  Nils Holmberg,et al.  Children’s Exposure to and Perceptions of Online Advertising , 2011 .