Why is it difficult to see in the fog? How stimulus contrast affects visual perception and visual memory

Processing visually degraded stimuli is a common experience. We struggle to find house keys on dim front porches, to decipher slides projected in overly bright seminar rooms, and to read 10th-generation photocopies. In this research, we focus specifically on stimuli that are degraded via reduction of stimuluscontrast and address two questions. First, why is it difficult to process low-contrast, as compared with high-contrast, stimuli? Second, is the effect of contrastfundamental in that its effect is independent of the stimulus being processed and the reason for processing the stimulus? We formally address and answer these questions within the context of a series of nested theories, each providing a successively stronger definition of what it means for contrast to affect perception and memory. To evaluate the theories, we carried out six experiments. Experiments 1 and 2 involved simple stimuli (randomly generated forms and digit strings), whereas Experiments 3–6 involved naturalistic pictures (faces, houses, and cityscapes). The stimuli were presented at two contrast levels and at varying exposure durations. The data from all the experiments allow the conclusion that some function of stimulus contrast combines multiplicatively with stimulus duration at a stage prior to that at which the nature of the stimulus and the reason for processing it are determined, and it is the result of this multiplicative combination that determines eventual memory performance. We describe a stronger version of this theory— the sensory response, information acquisition theory—which has at its core, the strong Bloch’s-law-like assumption of a fundamental visual system response that is proportional to the product of stimulus contrast and stimulus duration. This theory was, as it has been in the past, highly successful in accounting for memory for simple stimuli shown at short (i.e., shorter than an eye fixation) durations. However, it was less successful in accounting for data from short-duration naturalistic pictures and was entirely unsuccessful in accounting for data from naturalistic pictures shown at longer durations. We discuss (1) processing differences between short- and long-duration stimuli, (2) processing differences between simple stimuli, such as digits, and complex stimuli, such as pictures, (3) processing differences between biluminant stimuli (such as line drawings with only two luminance levels) and multiluminant stimuli (such as grayscale pictures with multiple luminance levels), and (4) Bloch’s law and a proposed generalization of the concept ofmetamers.

[1]  G Sperling,et al.  A signal-to-noise theory of the effects of luminance on picture memory: comment on Loftus. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[2]  D. Kahneman,et al.  THE TIME-INTENSITY RELATION IN VISUAL PERCEPTION AS A FUNCTION OF OBSERVER'S TASK. , 1964, Journal of experimental psychology.

[3]  Gurindar S. Sohi,et al.  Memory systems , 1996, CSUR.

[4]  A P Ginsburg,et al.  Suprathreshold processing of complex visual stimuli: evidence for linearity in contrast perception. , 1980, Science.

[5]  B B Murdock,et al.  TODAM2: a model for the storage and retrieval of item, associative, and serial-order information. , 1993, Psychological review.

[6]  B. Murdock A Theory for the Storage and Retrieval of Item and Associative Information. , 1982 .

[7]  E. Peli Contrast in complex images. , 1990, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[8]  G R Loftus,et al.  A theory of visual information acquisition and visual memory with special application to intensity-duration trade-offs. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  J. Kaswan,et al.  Stimulus exposure time, brightness, and spatial factors as determinants of visual perception. , 1963, Journal of experimental psychology.

[10]  R. Shiffrin,et al.  A retrieval model for both recognition and recall. , 1984, Psychological review.

[11]  Richard C. Atkinson,et al.  Human Memory: A Proposed System and its Control Processes , 1968, Psychology of Learning and Motivation.

[12]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[13]  G. Loftus,et al.  Sensory and cognitive components of visual information acquisition. , 1994, Psychological review.

[14]  G. Loftus,et al.  Binocular information acquisition and visual memory. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  Geoffrey R. Loftus,et al.  Evaluating forgetting curves. , 1985 .

[16]  M. A. Bouman,et al.  Spatial Modulation Transfer in the Human Eye , 1967 .

[17]  E. Hirshman,et al.  Perceptual interference improves explicit memory but does not enhance data-driven processing. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[18]  Wayne A. Wickelgren,et al.  Trace resistance and the decay of long-term memory. , 1972 .

[19]  Peter Herriot,et al.  Attributes of Memory , 1974 .

[20]  J. S. Nairne The mnemonic value of perceptual identification. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[21]  J. Palmer,et al.  Mechanisms of displacement discrimination with and without perceived movement. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  Geoffrey R. Loftus,et al.  Accounts of the confidence-accuracy relation in recognition memory , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[23]  John Palmer,et al.  Mechanisms of displacement discrimination with a visual reference , 1986, Vision Research.

[24]  C Bundesen,et al.  Visual selection from multielement displays: measuring and modeling effects of exposure duration. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  R. Bogartz,et al.  On the Meaning of Statistical Interactions. , 1976 .

[26]  David E. Rumelhart,et al.  A multicomponent theory of the perception of briefly exposed visual displays , 1970 .

[27]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[28]  Geoffrey R. Loftus,et al.  Learning-forgetting independence, unidimensional memory models, and feature models: comment on Bogartz (1990). , 1990 .

[29]  G. Loftus Picture perception: effects of luminance on available information and information-extraction rate. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[30]  D. Bamber State-trace analysis: A method of testing simple theories of causation , 1979 .

[31]  Geoffrey R. Loftus,et al.  Effects of Visual Degradation on Eye-Fixation Duration, Perceptual Processing, and Long-Term Visual Memory , 1992 .

[32]  N. J. Slamecka,et al.  The Generation Effect: Delineation of a Phenomenon , 1978 .

[33]  T A Busey,et al.  Providing a sensory basis for models of visual information acquisition , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[34]  David E. Irwin,et al.  On the Relations among Different Measures of Visible and Informational Persistence , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.

[35]  D. Norman,et al.  Acquisition and retention in short-term memory. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[36]  D. Jeffreys,et al.  Pattern-evoked potentials and Bloch's law , 1982, Vision Research.

[37]  John J. L. Morton,et al.  Interaction of information in word recognition. , 1969 .

[38]  K. Rayner Eye movements and visual cognition : scene perception and reading , 1992 .

[39]  N. Graham Visual Pattern Analyzers , 1989 .

[40]  K. Laughery,et al.  Recognition of human faces: effects of target exposure time, target position, pose position, and type of photograph. , 1971, The Journal of applied psychology.

[41]  Stephen A. Engel,et al.  Linearity across spatial frequency in object recognition , 1998, Vision Research.

[42]  Geoffrey R. Loftus,et al.  A theory of visual information acquisition and visual memory with special application to intensity-duration trade-offs. , 1994 .

[43]  Geoffrey R. Loftus,et al.  Consistency and Confoundings: Reply to Slamecka , 1985 .

[44]  G. Loftus,et al.  A front end to a theory of picture recognition , 1999, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[45]  Douglas L. Hintzman,et al.  MINERVA 2: A simulation model of human memory , 1984 .

[46]  G. Loftus On interpretation of interactions , 1978 .

[47]  G. Loftus Picture perception: effects of luminance on available information and information-extraction rate. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[48]  A. Paivio Mental imagery in associative learning and memory , 1969 .

[49]  H Spekreijse,et al.  Contrast evoked responses in man. , 1973, Vision research.

[50]  A. Gorea,et al.  New look at Bloch's law for contrast. , 1986, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[51]  W. A. Wickelgren Single-trace fragility theory of memory dynamics , 1974, Memory & cognition.

[52]  F. Craik,et al.  Levels of Pro-cessing: A Framework for Memory Research , 1975 .