Effect of sporting activity practice on susceptibility to motion sickness

The theory of visuo-vestibular conflict is the most commonly accepted to explain motion sickness. Visual, vestibular and proprioceptive afferences are involved in balance control and this function can be improved by physical and sporting activities (PSA). The purpose of the present survey was to investigate the relationships between motion sickness susceptibility (MSS) in adulthood and PSA, and especially proprioceptive PSA. A questionnaire concerning MSS and PSA was filled in by 1829 students (22.3+/-3.4 years of age, 799 males). Subjects having practised a sport before the age of 18 have less MSS than the other subjects (P<0.001). It should be noted that subjects who practised proprioceptive PSA before the age of 18 have less MSS than subjects who practised bioenergetic PSA before this age. By practising PSA, subjects are less dependent on visual input and use vestibular afferences better. A process of habituation can be involved in better managing conflicting sensory afferences reducing susceptibility to motion sickness. The practice of proprioceptive PSA develops the proprioceptive afferences and improves their treatment by the central nervous system. This additional appropriate input associated with an increase in vestibular weight compared to vision helps overcome visuo-vestibular conflict.

[1]  L. Yardley,et al.  Relationship between postural control and motion sickness in healthy subjects , 1998, Brain Research Bulletin.

[2]  R. Stern,et al.  Effects of gender of subjects and experimenter on susceptibility to motion sickness. , 1999, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[3]  J. Golding Motion sickness susceptibility questionnaire revised and its relationship to other forms of sickness , 1998, Brain Research Bulletin.

[4]  Gary E. Riccio,et al.  An Ecological Critique of the Sensory Conflict Theory of Motion Sickness , 1991 .

[5]  Stress measurements in normal and labyrinthine defective subjects in unusual force environments , 1965 .

[6]  C. Oman,et al.  Motion sickness: a synthesis and evaluation of the sensory conflict theory. , 1990, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[7]  M. Mira,et al.  Sex and seasickness on the Coral Sea , 1997, The Lancet.

[8]  Michael J Griffin,et al.  Visual field effects on motion sickness in cars. , 2004, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[9]  S. Hu,et al.  Gender differences in motion sickness history and susceptibility to optokinetic rotation-induced motion sickness. , 1999, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[10]  P. Bessou,et al.  Unusual vestibular and visual input in human dynamic balance as a motion sickness susceptibility test. , 1997, Aviation Space and Environmental Medicine.

[11]  N. Gadoth,et al.  The vestibulo-ocular reflex and seasickness susceptibility. , 1996, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[12]  L. Burrows,et al.  Evaluating sensory conflict and postural instability. theories of motion sickness , 1998, Brain Research Bulletin.

[13]  Y. Melamed,et al.  Clinical features of mal de debarquement: adaptation and habituation to sea conditions. , 1995, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[14]  T Mergner,et al.  Vestibular-neck interaction and transformation of sensory coordinates. , 1997, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[15]  J T Reason,et al.  Motion Sickness Adaptation: A Neural Mismatch Model 1 , 1978, Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine.

[16]  Avi Shupak,et al.  Computerized Dynamic Posturography and Seasickness Susceptibility , 1999, The Laryngoscope.

[17]  Gérome C. Gauchard,et al.  Beneficial effect of proprioceptive physical activities on balance control in elderly human subjects , 1999, Neuroscience Letters.

[18]  Philippe Perrin,et al.  Training improves the adaptation to changing visual conditions in maintaining human posture control in a test of sinusoidal oscillation of the support , 1998, Neuroscience Letters.

[19]  T. Fukuda Postural behaviour and motion sickness. , 1976, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[20]  Avi Shupak,et al.  Mal de Debarquement and Posture: Reduced Reliance on Vestibular and Visual Cues , 2004, The Laryngoscope.

[21]  H Kingma,et al.  Dizziness in discus throwers is related to motion sickness generated while spinning. , 2000, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[22]  Gérome C. Gauchard,et al.  Physical activity improves gaze and posture control in the elderly , 2003, Neuroscience Research.

[23]  Brice Isableu,et al.  Differential approach to strategies of segmental stabilisation in postural control , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  Y. Melamed,et al.  Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex as a Parameter of Seasickness Susceptibility , 1990, The Annals of otology, rhinology, and laryngology.

[25]  P. Perrin,et al.  Mood states and anxiety influence abilities to maintain balance control in healthy human subjects , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.

[26]  Gérome C. Gauchard,et al.  Physical and Sporting Activities Improve Vestibular Afferent Usage and Balance in Elderly Human Subjects , 2001, Gerontology.

[27]  J. May,et al.  The effects of age and sex on susceptibility to motion sickness. , 2001, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[28]  T. Stoffregen,et al.  An ecological Theory of Motion Sickness and Postural Instability , 1991 .

[29]  G. Gauchard,et al.  Influence of regular proprioceptive and bioenergetic physical activities on balance control in elderly women. , 2003, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[30]  Philippe Perrin,et al.  Judo, better than dance, develops sensorimotor adaptabilities involved in balance control. , 2002, Gait & posture.