An automated system for positive reinforcement training of group-housed macaque monkeys at breeding and research facilities

Highlights • An automated positive reinforcement training system for group-housed monkeys.• Animals can be trained with no food or fluid restriction.• Breeding facility results may predict subsequent research facility and lab performance.

[1]  A. Chamove Role or dominance in macaque response to novel objects , 1983 .

[2]  A C Roberts,et al.  Performance norms for a rhesus monkey neuropsychological testing battery: acquisition and long-term performance. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[3]  S. Schapiro,et al.  The Use of Positive Reinforcement Training Techniques to Enhance the Care, Management, and Welfare of Primates in the Laboratory , 2003, Journal of applied animal welfare science : JAAWS.

[4]  M. Spångberg,et al.  Positive reinforcement training in rhesus macaques—training progress as a result of training frequency , 2009, American journal of primatology.

[5]  K. Coleman,et al.  Temperament correlates with training success in adult rhesus macaques , 2005, American journal of primatology.

[6]  Yuan-Ye Ma,et al.  An automated food delivery system for behavioral and neurophysiological studies of learning and memory in freely moving monkeys , 2005, Behavior research methods.

[7]  V. Reinhardt Working With Rather Than Against Macaques During Blood Collection , 2003, Journal of applied animal welfare science : JAAWS.

[8]  W. Schultz,et al.  Refinement of the use of food and fluid control as motivational tools for macaques used in behavioural neuroscience research: Report of a Working Group of the NC3Rs , 2010, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[9]  B. McCowan,et al.  Human-directed contra-aggression training using positive reinforcement with single and multiple trainers for indoor-housed rhesus macaques , 2011 .

[10]  S. Schapiro,et al.  Training rhesus macaques for venipuncture using positive reinforcement techniques: a comparison with chimpanzees. , 2008, Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science : JAALAS.

[11]  E. Visalberghi,et al.  Identity concept learning in matching-to-sample tasks by tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) , 2010, Animal Cognition.

[12]  Steven A Boring,et al.  A novel food-delivery device for neurophysiological and neuropsychological studies in monkeys , 2001, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[13]  R. Hampton,et al.  Automated cognitive testing of monkeys in social groups yields results comparable to individual laboratory-based testing , 2013, Animal Cognition.

[14]  R. Young Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals , 2003 .

[15]  D A Washburn,et al.  Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), video tasks, and implications for stimulus-response spatial contiguity. , 1989, Journal of comparative psychology.

[16]  K. Lesch,et al.  Serotonin transporter gene polymorphism, differential early rearing, and behavior in rhesus monkey neonates , 2002, Molecular Psychiatry.

[17]  G. Sackett,et al.  A computer touch screen system and training procedure for use with primate infants: Results from pigtail monkeys (Macaca nemestrina). , 2008, Developmental psychobiology.

[18]  H. Buchanan-Smith,et al.  The psychology of control: Effects of control over supplementary light on welfare of marmosets , 2012 .

[19]  V. Melfi,et al.  Is training zoo animals enriching , 2013 .

[20]  S. Schapiro,et al.  Positive reinforcement training affects hematologic and serum chemistry values in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) , 2006, American journal of primatology.

[21]  Automated recording of individual performance and hand preference during joystick-task acquisition in group-living bonnet macaques (Macaca radiata). , 1994 .

[22]  M. Novak,et al.  A Rhesus Monkey Model of Self-Injury: Effects of Relocation Stress on Behavior and Neuroendocrine Function , 2008, Biological Psychiatry.

[23]  Chris T. Tromborg,et al.  Sources of stress in captivity , 2007 .

[24]  R. Thurston,et al.  Training to reliably obtain blood and urine samples from a diabetic chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) , 1996 .

[25]  A. Gail,et al.  A cage-based training, cognitive testing and enrichment system optimized for rhesus macaques in neuroscience research , 2017, Behavior research methods.

[26]  R. Spealman,et al.  Noninvasive technique for the repeated sampling of salivary free cortisol in awake, unrestrained squirrel monkeys , 2003, American journal of primatology.

[27]  M. Bloomsmith,et al.  Positive reinforcement training to enhance the voluntary movement of group‐housed chimpanzees within their enclosures , 1998 .

[28]  K. Westlund Training is enrichment—And beyond , 2014 .

[29]  B. McCowan,et al.  Implementing positive reinforcement animal training programs at primate laboratories , 2012 .

[30]  M. Bloomsmith,et al.  Positive reinforcement training to enhance the voluntary movement of group-housed sooty mangabeys (Cercocebus atys atys). , 2009, Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science : JAALAS.

[31]  J. Fagot,et al.  Automatic testing of cognitive performance in baboons maintained in social groups , 2009, Behavior research methods.

[32]  P. Honess,et al.  Enrichment and aggression in primates , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[33]  S. Schapiro,et al.  Physiological and Welfare Consequences of Transport, Relocation, and Acclimatization of Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). , 2012, Applied animal behaviour science.

[34]  M. Reite,et al.  Individual differences in Macaques' responses to stressors based on social and physiological factors: implications for primate welfare and research outcomes , 1995, Laboratory animals.

[35]  M. Reite,et al.  Long-term follow-up of previously separated pigtail macaques: group and individual differences in response to novel situations. , 1986, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[36]  K. Coleman,et al.  The use of positive reinforcement training to reduce stereotypic behavior in rhesus macaques. , 2010, Applied animal behaviour science.