Grooming Between Male Chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park. I. Partner Number and Diversity and Grooming Reciprocity

Allogrooming serves many social functions in primates. Grooming can help individuals to service social relationships generally, sometimes reciprocally, and may be particularly important in the development and maintenance of alliances. However, time constraints limit the number of partners with whom one individual can groom enough to maintain cooperative relationships. As a result, the size of its grooming network may reach an asymptote as the size of its group increases, and it may distribute its grooming less equally among potential partners. Chimpanzees live in multimale, fission-fusion communities; males are philopatric, and commonly associate and groom with each other. Males form within-community alliances that influence dominance rank and access to mates, and allies groom with each other regularly; males also cooperate in aggression between communities. The chimpanzee community at Ngogo, in Kibale National Park, Uganda, is unusually large and has more males than any other known community. Field data show that adult Ngogo males groomed far more with other adult males than with females or with adolescent males, in contrast to a previous report (Ghiglieri, 1984). Adolescent males groomed adults much more than the reverse; males groomed and were groomed by females about equally. Individual males groomed mostly with a small number of other males. On average, males at Ngogo had only slightly more male grooming partners overall and had the same number of important partners as those of males in a much smaller community in the Mahale National Park, Tanzania, and they distributed their grooming less equitably. These results fit those expected if limits on available grooming time cause males to have a loyalty problem as the number of potential grooming and alliance partners increases. Despite differences in the extent and equitability of their grooming networks, males at both Ngogo and Mahale showed reciprocity in grooming. Grooming reciprocity has been demonstrated for captive chimpanzee males, but the Ngogo findings are the first demonstrations of reciprocity in wild communities.

[1]  Susan Perry,et al.  Female‐female social relationships in wild white‐faced capuchin monkeys, Cebus capucinus , 1995 .

[2]  F. D. de Waal,et al.  Types of dominance in a chimpanzee colony. , 1980, Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology.

[3]  F. Waal Rank distance as a central feature of rhesus monkey social organization: a sociometric analysis , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[4]  Jeanne Altmann,et al.  Baboon mothers and infants , 1980 .

[5]  K. Stewart,et al.  The chimpanzees of Gombe: By Jane Goodall , 1987 .

[6]  Gary D. Schnell,et al.  Statistical comparison of proximity matrices: applications in animal behaviour , 1985, Animal Behaviour.

[7]  J. R. Walters Conflict and Cooperation , 2018, The Essence of Politics.

[8]  Charlotte K. Hemelrijk,et al.  Reciprocity and interchange of grooming and ‘support’ in captive chimpanzees , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[9]  Kazuhiko Hosaka,et al.  Great Ape Societies: Coalition strategies among adult male chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania , 1996 .

[10]  M. D. Bitetti Evidence for an important social role of allogrooming in a platyrrhine primate , 1997, Animal Behaviour.

[11]  F. D. de Waal,et al.  The similarity principle underlying social bonding among female rhesus monkeys. , 1986, Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology.

[12]  R. Wrangham,et al.  Sex differences in the behavioural ecology of chimpanzees in the Gombe National Park, Tanzania. , 1980, Journal of reproduction and fertility. Supplement.

[13]  John W. Pepper,et al.  General Gregariousness and Specific Social Preferences among Wild Chimpanzees , 1999, International Journal of Primatology.

[14]  Robin I. M. Dunbar Functional significance of social grooming in primates , 1991 .

[15]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Maternal time budgets of gelada baboons , 1988, Animal Behaviour.

[16]  Charlotte K. Hemelrijk,et al.  Sexual exchange relationships in captive chimpanzees? , 1992, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[17]  F. Waal,et al.  Sex differences in the formation of coalitions among chimpanzees , 1984 .

[18]  D. Watts,et al.  Grooming Between Male Chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park. II. Influence of Male Rank and Possible Competition for Partners , 2000, International Journal of Primatology.

[19]  L. Luttrell,et al.  Mechanisms of social reciprocity in three primate species: Symmetrical relationship characteristics or cognition? , 1988 .

[20]  Robin I. M. Dunbar Reproductive Decisions: An Economic Analysis of Gelada Baboon Social Strategies , 1984 .

[21]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Time: a hidden constraint on the behavioural ecology of baboons , 1992, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[22]  S. P. Henzi,et al.  Fission and troop size in a mountain baboon population , 1997, Animal Behaviour.

[23]  Charlotte K. Hemelrijk,et al.  A matrix partial correlation test used in investigations of reciprocity and other social interaction patterns at group level , 1990 .

[24]  Toshisada Nishida,et al.  The social group of wild chimpanzees in the Mahali Mountains , 1968, Primates.

[25]  L. Parr,et al.  Grooming down the hierarchy: allogrooming in captive brown capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella , 1997, Animal Behaviour.

[26]  A. Whiten,et al.  Priority of access and grooming patterns of females in a large and a small group of olive baboons , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[27]  Robin I. M. Dunbar Primate social systems , 1987 .

[28]  D. Watts Coalitionary mate guarding by male chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda , 1998, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[29]  Marina Cords,et al.  Machiavellian Intelligence II: Friendships, alliances, reciprocity and repair , 1997 .

[30]  C. Schaik,et al.  Male Bonds: Afilliative Relationships Among Nonhuman Primate Males , 1994 .

[31]  Jane Goodall,et al.  Behaviour of free-living chimpanzees , 1966 .

[32]  Thomas M. Butynski,et al.  Comparative Ecology of Blue Monkeys (Cercopithecus Mitis) in High‐ and Low‐Density Subpopulations , 1990 .

[33]  Jonathan Scott Friedlaender,et al.  Partial correlation of distance matrices in studies of population structure. , 1987, American journal of physical anthropology.

[34]  F. D. Waal,et al.  Food sharing and reciprocal obligations among chimpanzees , 1989 .

[35]  M. P. Ghiglieri The chimpanzees of Kibale Forest : a field study of ecology and social structure , 1984 .

[36]  T. Nishida,et al.  Alpha status and agonistic alliance in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) , 1983, Primates.

[37]  Charlotte K. Hemelrijk,et al.  Models of, and tests for, reciprocity, unidirectionality and other social interaction patterns at a group level , 1990, Animal Behaviour.

[38]  J. Mitani,et al.  Demographic influences on the hunting behavior of chimpanzees. , 1999, American journal of physical anthropology.

[39]  Charlotte K. Hemelrijk,et al.  Philopatry, male presence and grooming reciprocation among female primates: a comparative perspective , 1998, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.