Strategies optimize the detection of motion transients.

Strategies are implicitly formed when a task is consistent and can be used to improve performance. To investigate how strategies can alter perceptual performance, I trained animals in a reaction time (RT) detection task in which the probability of a fixed duration motion pulse appearing varied over time in a consistent manner. Consistent with previous studies suggesting the implicit representation of task timing, I found that RTs were inversely related to the probability of the pulse appearing and decreased with training. I then inferred the sensory integration underlying responses using behavioral reverse correlation analysis. This analysis revealed that training and anticipation optimized detection by improving the correlation between sensory integration and the spatiotemporal extent of the motion pulse. Moreover, I found that these improvements in sensory integration could largely explain observed changes in the distribution of RT with training and anticipation. These results suggest that training can increase detection performance by optimizing sensory integration according to implicitly formed representations of the likelihood and nature of the stimulus.

[1]  J. Maunsell,et al.  Attentional Modulation of Behavioral Performance and Neuronal Responses in Middle Temporal and Ventral Intraparietal Areas of Macaque Monkey , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  M. Shadlen,et al.  A representation of the hazard rate of elapsed time in macaque area LIP , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  L. Kaufman,et al.  Handbook of perception and human performance , 1986 .

[4]  D. Robinson,et al.  A METHOD OF MEASURING EYE MOVEMENT USING A SCLERAL SEARCH COIL IN A MAGNETIC FIELD. , 1963, IEEE transactions on bio-medical engineering.

[5]  James T. Todd,et al.  The effects of spatiotemporal integration on maximum displacement thresholds for the detection of coherent motion , 1995, Vision Research.

[6]  D. Robinson,et al.  Covert orienting of attention in macaques. I. Effects of behavioral context. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Attentional modulation of visual motion processing in cortical areas MT and MST , 1996, Nature.

[8]  S P Tripathy,et al.  Insights into motion perception by observer modeling. , 1998, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[9]  D. Burr,et al.  Large receptive fields for optic flow detection in humans , 1998, Vision Research.

[10]  J. Movshon,et al.  The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[11]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Attention and performance. , 2001, Annual review of psychology.

[12]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .

[13]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Unconscious Masked Priming Depends on Temporal Attention , 2002, Psychological science.

[14]  Anna Christina Nobre,et al.  Orienting attention to instants in time , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  T. Albright,et al.  Efficient Discrimination of Temporal Patterns by Motion-Sensitive Neurons in Primate Visual Cortex , 1998, Neuron.

[16]  Christof Koch,et al.  Temporal Precision of Spike Trains in Extrastriate Cortex of the Behaving Macaque Monkey , 1999, Neural Computation.

[17]  R. Carpenter,et al.  The influence of urgency on decision time , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  A. Watson,et al.  The optimal motion stimulus , 1995, Vision Research.

[19]  J. Movshon,et al.  Adaptive Temporal Integration of Motion in Direction-Selective Neurons in Macaque Visual Cortex , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.

[21]  Richard J. A. van Wezel,et al.  The motion reverse correlation (MRC) method: A linear systems approach in the motion domain , 2003, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[22]  Miguel P Eckstein,et al.  The time course of visual information accrual guiding eye movement decisions. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[23]  M. Bravo,et al.  The role of attention in different visual-search tasks , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[24]  D. Burr,et al.  Temporal integration of optic flow, measured by contrast and coherence thresholds , 2001, Vision Research.

[25]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  A diffusion model account of masking in two-choice letter identification. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  R. Ratcliff Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[27]  H. B. Barlow,et al.  What does the eye see best? , 1983, Nature.

[28]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Response of Neurons in the Lateral Intraparietal Area during a Combined Visual Discrimination Reaction Time Task , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  Christopher C. Pack,et al.  Dynamic properties of neurons in cortical area MT in alert and anaesthetized macaque monkeys , 2001, Nature.

[30]  D. Whitteridge Movements of the eyes R. H. S. Carpenter, Pion Ltd, London (1977), 420 pp., $27.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.

[31]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Detection of changes in speed and direction of motion: Reaction time analysis , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[32]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Temporal properties of visual motion signals for the initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  W. Newsome,et al.  Microstimulation in visual area MT: effects on direction discrimination performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[34]  R. Duncan Luce,et al.  Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization , 1986 .

[35]  Geoffrey M. Ghose,et al.  Attentional modulation in visual cortex depends on task timing , 2002, Nature.

[36]  D C Van Essen,et al.  Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  M. Shadlen,et al.  A role for neural integrators in perceptual decision making. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[38]  David J. Heeger,et al.  Spatiotemporal mechanisms for detecting and identifying image features in human vision , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  A. Nobre,et al.  Orienting attention in time: behavioural and neuroanatomical distinction between exogenous and endogenous shifts , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  R. Carpenter,et al.  Contrast, Probability, and Saccadic Latency Evidence for Independence of Detection and Decision , 2004, Current Biology.

[41]  R. Ratcliff Putting noise into neurophysiological models of simple decision making , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[42]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Temporal integration of random dot apparent motion information in human central vision , 1994, Vision Research.

[43]  Christof Koch,et al.  Detecting and Estimating Signals over Noisy and Unreliable Synapses: Information-Theoretic Analysis , 2001, Neural Computation.

[44]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Spatial summation and its interaction with the temporal integration mechanism in human motion perception , 1994, Vision Research.

[45]  D L Robinson,et al.  Covert orienting of attention in macaques. III. Contributions of the superior colliculus. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  J. Koenderink,et al.  Spatiotemporal integration in the detection of coherent motion , 1984, Vision Research.

[47]  E. Seidemann,et al.  Effect of spatial attention on the responses of area MT neurons. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  Erich E. Sutter,et al.  The Fast m-Transform: A Fast Computation of Cross-Correlations with Binary m-Sequences , 1991, SIAM J. Comput..

[49]  D. Burr,et al.  Dependency of reaction times to motion onset on luminance and chromatic contrast , 2001, Vision Research.

[50]  R. Carpenter,et al.  Movements of the Eyes , 1978 .

[51]  A. Gorea,et al.  New look at Bloch's law for contrast. , 1986, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[52]  R. H. S. Carpenter,et al.  Neural computation of log likelihood in control of saccadic eye movements , 1995, Nature.

[53]  J. Lappin,et al.  The detection of coherence in moving random-dot patterns , 1976, Vision Research.

[54]  A. Watson Probability summation over time , 1979, Vision Research.

[55]  S P McKee,et al.  Stimulus configuration determines the detectability of motion signals in noise. , 2000, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[56]  M. Chun,et al.  Top-Down Attentional Guidance Based on Implicit Learning of Visual Covariation , 1999 .

[57]  M. Chun,et al.  Temporal contextual cuing of visual attention. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[58]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Neural correlates of a decision in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the macaque , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[59]  Miguel P Eckstein,et al.  The footprints of visual attention in the Posner cueing paradigm revealed by classification images. , 2002, Journal of vision.

[60]  O. Braddick,et al.  The temporal integration and resolution of velocity signals , 1991, Vision Research.

[61]  W. Newsome,et al.  Deficits in visual motion processing following ibotenic acid lesions of the middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[62]  Brian J. Murphy,et al.  Summation and discrimination of gratings moving in opposite directions , 1980, Vision Research.

[63]  H Barlow,et al.  Correspondence Noise and Signal Pooling in the Detection of Coherent Visual Motion , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[64]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  An analysis of the temporal integration mechanism in human motion perception , 1994, Vision Research.

[65]  R. Shapley,et al.  The use of m-sequences in the analysis of visual neurons: Linear receptive field properties , 1997, Visual Neuroscience.

[66]  W. Newsome,et al.  Motion selectivity in macaque visual cortex. II. Spatiotemporal range of directional interactions in MT and V1. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.