EVOLVING SOCIAL CONSTRAINTS ON INDIVIDUAL CONCEPTUAL REPRESENTATIONS

The paper deals with how constraints on conceptual representations evolve through processes of knowledge shar- ing. We describe pragmatic settings of referential communication and provide a model of how names, nouns and adjectives emerge through a process of abstraction. In situations of referential communication, the contrast class - the set that intended referents must be distinguished from - is important for determining the degree of specification of referential utter- ances. Two processing strategies involving contrast classes are proposed that are connected to the nominal and adjectival levels of abstraction. Certain cognitive representational skills are needed to be able to assess a contrast class in a commu- nicative situation. We propose three communicative strategies that correspond to different assessments of the relevant con- trast class.

[1]  M. Cole,et al.  Cognitive Development: Its Cultural and Social Foundations , 1976 .

[2]  Robert P. Worden Primate Social Intelligence , 1996, Cogn. Sci..

[3]  W. Hanks Referential Practice: Language and Lived Space among the Maya , 1990 .

[4]  S. Harnad Categorical Perception: The Groundwork of Cognition , 1990 .

[5]  Sven Ove Hansson,et al.  Logic for a Change: Essays dedicated to Sten lindström on the occasion of his fiftieth birthday , 1995 .

[6]  Donald Favareau The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain , 1998 .

[7]  T. Givón,et al.  Syntax: A Functional-Typological Introduction , 1982 .

[8]  Eleanor Rosch,et al.  Principles of Categorization , 1978 .

[9]  W. Levelt,et al.  Speaking: From Intention to Articulation , 1990 .

[10]  S. Winter EVOLUTION, CATEGORIZATION AND VALUES , 1998 .

[11]  Arne Naess,et al.  Interpretation and preciseness : a contribution to the theory of communication , 1953 .

[12]  Ivy Yee-Man Lau,et al.  Some Cognitive Consequences of Communication , 1998 .

[13]  Robert L. Goldstone,et al.  The development of features in object concepts , 1998, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[14]  R. Krauss,et al.  Social and Nonsocial Speech , 1977 .

[15]  Peter Gardenfors Concept representations and nonmonotic inferences , 1996 .

[16]  John D. Coley,et al.  Perspectives on language and thought: Language and categorization: The acquisition of natural kind terms , 1991 .

[17]  Jooyong Park,et al.  A Causal-Power Theory of Focal Sets , 1996 .

[18]  S. Harnad Category Induction and Representation , 1987 .

[19]  László Pólos,et al.  Knowledge Representation and Reasoning Under Uncertainty , 1992, Lecture Notes in Computer Science.

[20]  S. Carey Conceptual Change in Childhood , 1985 .

[21]  Robert E. Kraut,et al.  Intellectual Teamwork: Social and Technological Foundations of Cooperative Work , 1990 .

[22]  D R Olson,et al.  Language and thought: aspects of a cognitive theory of semantics. , 1970, Psychological review.

[23]  Peter Gärdenfors,et al.  Linguistic Modality as Expressions of Social Power , 1995, Nordic Journal of Linguistics.

[24]  S. Winter,et al.  DIALOGUE DYNAMICS, VIOLIN STRINGS, AND THE PRAGMATICS- SEMANTICS CONTINUUM , 1998 .

[25]  David R. Olson,et al.  Interpreting Texts and Interpreting Nature: The Effects of Literacy on Hermeneutics and Epistemology. , 1988 .

[26]  Linda B. Smith From global similarities to kinds of similarities: the construction of dimensions in development , 1989 .

[27]  Susan R. Fussell,et al.  Mutual knowledge and communicative effectiveness , 1990 .

[28]  R. Rommetveit On Message Structure: A Framework for the Study of Language and Communication , 1976, Dance Research Journal.

[29]  John H. Holland,et al.  Induction: Processes of Inference, Learning, and Discovery , 1987, IEEE Expert.

[30]  Peter Gärdenfors The Role of Expectations in Reasoning , 1992, Logic at Work.

[31]  J. Freyd Shareability: The Social Psychology of Epistemology* , 1983 .