A Neural Model of How The Brain Represents and Compares Numbers

Many psychophysical experiments have shown that the representation of numbers and numerical quantities in humans and animals is related to number magnitude. A neural network model is proposed to quantitatively simulate error rates in quantification and numerical comparison tasks, and reaction times for number priming and numerical assessment and comparison tasks. Transient responses to inputs are integrated before they activate an ordered spatial map that selectively responds to the number of events in a sequence. The dynamics of numerical comparison are encoded in activity pattern changes within this spatial map. Such changes cause a "directional comparison wave" whose properties mimic data about numerical comparison. These model mechanisms are variants of neural mechanisms that have elsewhere been used to explain data about motion perception, attention shifts, and target tracking. Thus, the present model suggests how numerical representations may have emerged as specializations of more primitive mechanisms in the cortical Where processing stream.

[1]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  A neural architecture for visual motion perception: group and element apparent motion , 1989, International 1989 Joint Conference on Neural Networks.

[2]  M McCloskey,et al.  Representing and using numerical information. , 1995, The American psychologist.

[3]  S. Grossberg,et al.  Neural dynamics of motion processing and speed discrimination , 1998, Vision Research.

[4]  Jamie I. D. Campbell,et al.  Chapter 12 Cognitive Number Processing: An Encoding-Complex Perspective , 1992 .

[5]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[6]  M. McCloskey Cognitive mechanisms in numerical processing: Evidence from acquired dyscalculia , 1992, Cognition.

[7]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Cerebral Pathways for Calculation: Double Dissociation between Rote Verbal and Quantitative Knowledge of Arithmetic , 1997, Cortex.

[8]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Object and spatial visual working memory activate separate neural systems in human cortex. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .

[10]  M. Brysbaert Arabic number reading: On the nature of the numerical scale and the origin of phonological recoding. , 1995 .

[11]  E. Spelke,et al.  Detection of intermodal numerical correspondences by human infants. , 1983, Science.

[12]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Development of Elementary Numerical Abilities: A Neuronal Model , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  S. Grossberg,et al.  Cortical dynamics of form and motion integration: Persistence, apparent motion, and illusory contours , 1996, Vision Research.

[14]  H. Heinze,et al.  On the interaction of numerical and size information in digit comparison: a behavioral and event-related potential study , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  K. Briand,et al.  Priming single digit numbers: automatic spreading activation dissipates as a function of semantic distance , 1986 .

[16]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  How is a moving target continuously tracked behind occluding cover , 1997 .

[17]  S. Grossberg,et al.  Cortical dynamics of visual motion perception: short-range and long-range apparent motion. , 1992, Psychological review.

[18]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Dissociable mechanisms of subitizing and counting: Neuropsychological evidence from simultanagnosic patients. , 1994 .

[19]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  Neural dynamics of motion grouping: from aperture ambiguity to object speed and direction , 1997 .

[20]  Jacky Emmerton,et al.  Pigeons’ serial ordering of numerosity with visual arrays , 1997 .

[21]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Cerebral activations during number multiplication and comparison: a PET study , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  David A. Washburn,et al.  Ordinal Judgments of Numerical Symbols by Macaques (Macaca Mulatta) , 1991, Psychological science.

[23]  Aijaz A. Baloch,et al.  A neural model of high-level motion processing: Line motion and formotion dynamics , 1997, Vision Research.

[24]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  Karen Wynn,et al.  Psychological foundations of number: numerical competence in human infants , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  R. Church,et al.  A mode control model of counting and timing processes. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[27]  E. Deibert,et al.  Neural pathways in tactile object recognition , 1999, Neurology.

[28]  S. Link Modeling imageless thought: The relative judgment theory of numerical comparisons ☆ , 1990 .

[29]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  A Neural Theory of Punishment and Avoidance, II: Quantitative Theory , 1972 .

[30]  S. Grossberg,et al.  A Neural Model of Multimodal Adaptive Saccadic Eye Movement Control by Superior Colliculus , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Event-related fMRI analysis of the cerebral circuit for number comparison. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[32]  F Mechner,et al.  Sequential dependencies of the lengths of consecutive response runs. , 1958, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[33]  G. Mandler,et al.  Subitizing: an analysis of its component processes. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[34]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. , 1998 .

[35]  M. E. Bitterman,et al.  Learning in honeybees as a function of amount and frequency of reward , 1988 .

[36]  John M. Parkman,et al.  Temporal aspects of digit and letter inequality judgments. , 1971, Journal of Experimental Psychology.

[37]  R. M. Siegel,et al.  Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. , 1985, Science.

[38]  Mark H. Ashcraft,et al.  Children’s Knowledge of Simple Arithmetic: A Developmental Model and Simulation , 1987 .

[39]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Knowing where and getting there: a human navigation network. , 1998, Science.

[40]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  The syllable-length effect in number processing is task-dependent , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[41]  M. Ashcraft Cognitive arithmetic: A review of data and theory , 1992, Cognition.

[42]  Jamie I. D. Campbell,et al.  Integrated versus modular theories of number skills and acalculia , 1991, Brain and Cognition.

[43]  S. Dehaene Varieties of numerical abilities , 1992, Cognition.