A Neural Model of How The Brain Represents and Compares Numbers
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Stephen Grossberg,et al. A neural architecture for visual motion perception: group and element apparent motion , 1989, International 1989 Joint Conference on Neural Networks.
[2] M McCloskey,et al. Representing and using numerical information. , 1995, The American psychologist.
[3] S. Grossberg,et al. Neural dynamics of motion processing and speed discrimination , 1998, Vision Research.
[4] Jamie I. D. Campbell,et al. Chapter 12 Cognitive Number Processing: An Encoding-Complex Perspective , 1992 .
[5] M. Goodale,et al. Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.
[6] M. McCloskey. Cognitive mechanisms in numerical processing: Evidence from acquired dyscalculia , 1992, Cognition.
[7] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Cerebral Pathways for Calculation: Double Dissociation between Rote Verbal and Quantitative Knowledge of Arithmetic , 1997, Cortex.
[8] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. Object and spatial visual working memory activate separate neural systems in human cortex. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.
[9] S. Dehaene,et al. The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .
[10] M. Brysbaert. Arabic number reading: On the nature of the numerical scale and the origin of phonological recoding. , 1995 .
[11] E. Spelke,et al. Detection of intermodal numerical correspondences by human infants. , 1983, Science.
[12] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Development of Elementary Numerical Abilities: A Neuronal Model , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[13] S. Grossberg,et al. Cortical dynamics of form and motion integration: Persistence, apparent motion, and illusory contours , 1996, Vision Research.
[14] H. Heinze,et al. On the interaction of numerical and size information in digit comparison: a behavioral and event-related potential study , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[15] K. Briand,et al. Priming single digit numbers: automatic spreading activation dissipates as a function of semantic distance , 1986 .
[16] Stephen Grossberg,et al. How is a moving target continuously tracked behind occluding cover , 1997 .
[17] S. Grossberg,et al. Cortical dynamics of visual motion perception: short-range and long-range apparent motion. , 1992, Psychological review.
[18] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Dissociable mechanisms of subitizing and counting: Neuropsychological evidence from simultanagnosic patients. , 1994 .
[19] Stephen Grossberg,et al. Neural dynamics of motion grouping: from aperture ambiguity to object speed and direction , 1997 .
[20] Jacky Emmerton,et al. Pigeons’ serial ordering of numerosity with visual arrays , 1997 .
[21] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Cerebral activations during number multiplication and comparison: a PET study , 1996, Neuropsychologia.
[22] David A. Washburn,et al. Ordinal Judgments of Numerical Symbols by Macaques (Macaca Mulatta) , 1991, Psychological science.
[23] Aijaz A. Baloch,et al. A neural model of high-level motion processing: Line motion and formotion dynamics , 1997, Vision Research.
[24] S. Dehaene,et al. Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[25] Karen Wynn,et al. Psychological foundations of number: numerical competence in human infants , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[26] R. Church,et al. A mode control model of counting and timing processes. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[27] E. Deibert,et al. Neural pathways in tactile object recognition , 1999, Neurology.
[28] S. Link. Modeling imageless thought: The relative judgment theory of numerical comparisons ☆ , 1990 .
[29] Stephen Grossberg,et al. A Neural Theory of Punishment and Avoidance, II: Quantitative Theory , 1972 .
[30] S. Grossberg,et al. A Neural Model of Multimodal Adaptive Saccadic Eye Movement Control by Superior Colliculus , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[31] S. Dehaene,et al. Event-related fMRI analysis of the cerebral circuit for number comparison. , 1999, Neuroreport.
[32] F Mechner,et al. Sequential dependencies of the lengths of consecutive response runs. , 1958, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[33] G. Mandler,et al. Subitizing: an analysis of its component processes. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[34] S. Dehaene,et al. The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. , 1998 .
[35] M. E. Bitterman,et al. Learning in honeybees as a function of amount and frequency of reward , 1988 .
[36] John M. Parkman,et al. Temporal aspects of digit and letter inequality judgments. , 1971, Journal of Experimental Psychology.
[37] R. M. Siegel,et al. Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. , 1985, Science.
[38] Mark H. Ashcraft,et al. Children’s Knowledge of Simple Arithmetic: A Developmental Model and Simulation , 1987 .
[39] Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al. Knowing where and getting there: a human navigation network. , 1998, Science.
[40] Marc Brysbaert,et al. The syllable-length effect in number processing is task-dependent , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.
[41] M. Ashcraft. Cognitive arithmetic: A review of data and theory , 1992, Cognition.
[42] Jamie I. D. Campbell,et al. Integrated versus modular theories of number skills and acalculia , 1991, Brain and Cognition.
[43] S. Dehaene. Varieties of numerical abilities , 1992, Cognition.