What is a language made out of

[1]  J. Mandler,et al.  Drinking and driving don't mix: inductive generalization in infancy , 1996, Cognition.

[2]  Letitia R. Naigles,et al.  The use of multiple frames in verb learning via syntactic bootstrapping , 1996, Cognition.

[3]  James L. Morgan,et al.  Signal to syntax : bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition , 1996 .

[4]  Timothy A. Cartwright,et al.  Distributional regularity and phonotactics are useful for early lexical acquisition , 1996 .

[5]  Letitia R. Naigles,et al.  Input to verb learning: Evidence for the plausibility of syntactic bootstrapping , 1995 .

[6]  Paul Bloom,et al.  Syntactic cues in the acquisition of collective nouns , 1995, Cognition.

[7]  Elizabeth Bates,et al.  A cross-linguistic study of early lexical development , 1995 .

[8]  Alison Gopnik,et al.  Names, relational words, and cognitive development in English and Korean speakers: Nouns are not always learned before verbs. , 1995 .

[9]  K. Demuth,et al.  Proceedings of the North East Linguistic Society , 1995 .

[10]  Cynthia L Fisher,et al.  Structure and meaning in the verb lexicon: Input for a syntax-aided verb learning procedure , 1994 .

[11]  How mothers teach basic-level and situation-restricted count nouns. , 1994 .

[12]  M. R. Brent,et al.  Surface cues and robust inference as a basis for the early acquisition of subcategorization frames , 1994 .

[13]  M. H. Kelly,et al.  Domain-general abilities applied to domain-specific tasks: Sensitivity to probabilities in perception, cognition, and language☆ , 1994 .

[14]  P. Bloom,et al.  Possible names: The role of syntax-semantics mappings in the acquisition of nominals , 1994 .

[15]  S. Pinker How could a child use verb syntax to learn verb semantics , 1994 .

[16]  E. Markman,et al.  When it is better to receive than to give: Syntactic and conceptual constraints on vocabulary growth , 1994 .

[17]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  When prosody fails to cue syntactic structure: 9-month-olds' sensitivity to phonological versus syntactic phrases , 1994, Cognition.

[18]  B. Landau,et al.  “What” and “where” in spatial language and spatial cognition , 1993 .

[19]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Joint attention on actions: acquiring verbs in ostensive and non-ostensive contexts , 1992, Journal of Child Language.

[20]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Infants' contribution to the achievement of joint reference. , 1991, Child development.

[21]  David R. Dowty Thematic proto-roles and argument selection , 1991 .

[22]  Cynthia Fisher,et al.  On the semantic content of subcategorization frames , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[23]  M. H. Kelly,et al.  Phonological information for grammatical category assignments , 1991 .

[24]  E. Spelke,et al.  Ontological categories guide young children's inductions of word meaning: Object terms and substance terms , 1991, Cognition.

[25]  Letitia R. Naigles,et al.  Children use syntax to learn verb meanings , 1990, Journal of Child Language.

[26]  Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  A moment of silence: How the prosodic cues in motherese might assist language learning , 1986 .

[27]  L. Gleitman,et al.  Language and Experience: Evidence from the Blind Child , 1988 .

[28]  E. Clark,et al.  On the logic of contrast , 1988, Journal of Child Language.

[29]  E. Markman,et al.  Children's use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words , 1988, Cognitive Psychology.

[30]  Elissa L Newport,et al.  Structural packaging in the input to language learning: Contributions of prosodic and morphological marking of phrases to the acquisition of language , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[31]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Clauses are perceptual units for young infants , 1987, Cognition.

[32]  Steven Pinker,et al.  Language learnability and language development , 1985 .

[33]  C. L. Baker,et al.  The Logical problem of language acquisition , 1984 .

[34]  E. Markman,et al.  Children's sensitivity to constraints on word meaning: Taxonomic versus thematic relations , 1984, Cognitive Psychology.

[35]  S Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Gestural communication in deaf children: the effects and noneffects of parental input on early language development. , 1984, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[36]  Elizabeth F. Shipley,et al.  Mothers' use of superordinate category terms , 1983, Journal of Child Language.

[37]  L. Bloom Tensions in psycholinguistics. , 1983, Science.

[38]  Eric Wanner,et al.  Language acquisition: the state of the art , 1982 .

[39]  G. Miller,et al.  Linguistic theory and psychological reality , 1982 .

[40]  M. Garrett,et al.  Perceptual knowledge of objects in infancy , 1982 .

[41]  Elissa L. Newport,et al.  The role of constituent structure in the induction of an artificial language , 1981 .

[42]  A. Ninio Ostensive definition in vocabulary teaching , 1980, Journal of Child Language.

[43]  L. Rescorla,et al.  Overextension in early language development , 1980, Journal of Child Language.

[44]  S. Carey The child as word learner , 1978 .

[45]  Dedre Gentner,et al.  On Relational Meaning: The Acquisition of Verb Meaning. Technical Report No. 78. , 1977 .

[46]  John Macnamara,et al.  What's in a name? A study of how children learn common and proper names. , 1974 .

[47]  P. D. Eimas,et al.  Speech Perception in Infants , 1971, Science.

[48]  L. Bloom Language Development: Form and Function in Emerging Grammars , 1970 .

[49]  Roderick A. Jacobs,et al.  Readings in English transformational grammar , 1970 .

[50]  Roger S. Brown,et al.  Linguistic determinism and the part of speech. , 1957, Journal of abnormal psychology.