What is a language made out of
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] J. Mandler,et al. Drinking and driving don't mix: inductive generalization in infancy , 1996, Cognition.
[2] Letitia R. Naigles,et al. The use of multiple frames in verb learning via syntactic bootstrapping , 1996, Cognition.
[3] James L. Morgan,et al. Signal to syntax : bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition , 1996 .
[4] Timothy A. Cartwright,et al. Distributional regularity and phonotactics are useful for early lexical acquisition , 1996 .
[5] Letitia R. Naigles,et al. Input to verb learning: Evidence for the plausibility of syntactic bootstrapping , 1995 .
[6] Paul Bloom,et al. Syntactic cues in the acquisition of collective nouns , 1995, Cognition.
[7] Elizabeth Bates,et al. A cross-linguistic study of early lexical development , 1995 .
[8] Alison Gopnik,et al. Names, relational words, and cognitive development in English and Korean speakers: Nouns are not always learned before verbs. , 1995 .
[9] K. Demuth,et al. Proceedings of the North East Linguistic Society , 1995 .
[10] Cynthia L Fisher,et al. Structure and meaning in the verb lexicon: Input for a syntax-aided verb learning procedure , 1994 .
[11] How mothers teach basic-level and situation-restricted count nouns. , 1994 .
[12] M. R. Brent,et al. Surface cues and robust inference as a basis for the early acquisition of subcategorization frames , 1994 .
[13] M. H. Kelly,et al. Domain-general abilities applied to domain-specific tasks: Sensitivity to probabilities in perception, cognition, and language☆ , 1994 .
[14] P. Bloom,et al. Possible names: The role of syntax-semantics mappings in the acquisition of nominals , 1994 .
[15] S. Pinker. How could a child use verb syntax to learn verb semantics , 1994 .
[16] E. Markman,et al. When it is better to receive than to give: Syntactic and conceptual constraints on vocabulary growth , 1994 .
[17] P. Jusczyk,et al. When prosody fails to cue syntactic structure: 9-month-olds' sensitivity to phonological versus syntactic phrases , 1994, Cognition.
[18] B. Landau,et al. “What” and “where” in spatial language and spatial cognition , 1993 .
[19] M. Tomasello,et al. Joint attention on actions: acquiring verbs in ostensive and non-ostensive contexts , 1992, Journal of Child Language.
[20] Dare A. Baldwin,et al. Infants' contribution to the achievement of joint reference. , 1991, Child development.
[21] David R. Dowty. Thematic proto-roles and argument selection , 1991 .
[22] Cynthia Fisher,et al. On the semantic content of subcategorization frames , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.
[23] M. H. Kelly,et al. Phonological information for grammatical category assignments , 1991 .
[24] E. Spelke,et al. Ontological categories guide young children's inductions of word meaning: Object terms and substance terms , 1991, Cognition.
[25] Letitia R. Naigles,et al. Children use syntax to learn verb meanings , 1990, Journal of Child Language.
[26] Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al. A moment of silence: How the prosodic cues in motherese might assist language learning , 1986 .
[27] L. Gleitman,et al. Language and Experience: Evidence from the Blind Child , 1988 .
[28] E. Clark,et al. On the logic of contrast , 1988, Journal of Child Language.
[29] E. Markman,et al. Children's use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words , 1988, Cognitive Psychology.
[30] Elissa L Newport,et al. Structural packaging in the input to language learning: Contributions of prosodic and morphological marking of phrases to the acquisition of language , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.
[31] P. Jusczyk,et al. Clauses are perceptual units for young infants , 1987, Cognition.
[32] Steven Pinker,et al. Language learnability and language development , 1985 .
[33] C. L. Baker,et al. The Logical problem of language acquisition , 1984 .
[34] E. Markman,et al. Children's sensitivity to constraints on word meaning: Taxonomic versus thematic relations , 1984, Cognitive Psychology.
[35] S Goldin-Meadow,et al. Gestural communication in deaf children: the effects and noneffects of parental input on early language development. , 1984, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.
[36] Elizabeth F. Shipley,et al. Mothers' use of superordinate category terms , 1983, Journal of Child Language.
[37] L. Bloom. Tensions in psycholinguistics. , 1983, Science.
[38] Eric Wanner,et al. Language acquisition: the state of the art , 1982 .
[39] G. Miller,et al. Linguistic theory and psychological reality , 1982 .
[40] M. Garrett,et al. Perceptual knowledge of objects in infancy , 1982 .
[41] Elissa L. Newport,et al. The role of constituent structure in the induction of an artificial language , 1981 .
[42] A. Ninio. Ostensive definition in vocabulary teaching , 1980, Journal of Child Language.
[43] L. Rescorla,et al. Overextension in early language development , 1980, Journal of Child Language.
[44] S. Carey. The child as word learner , 1978 .
[45] Dedre Gentner,et al. On Relational Meaning: The Acquisition of Verb Meaning. Technical Report No. 78. , 1977 .
[46] John Macnamara,et al. What's in a name? A study of how children learn common and proper names. , 1974 .
[47] P. D. Eimas,et al. Speech Perception in Infants , 1971, Science.
[48] L. Bloom. Language Development: Form and Function in Emerging Grammars , 1970 .
[49] Roderick A. Jacobs,et al. Readings in English transformational grammar , 1970 .
[50] Roger S. Brown,et al. Linguistic determinism and the part of speech. , 1957, Journal of abnormal psychology.