The three arousal model: implications of gray's two-factor learning theory for heart rate, electrodermal activity, and psychopathy.

Gray's two-factor learning theory postulates a behavioral activation system (BAS), a behavioral inhibition system (BIS), and a nonspecific arousal system receiving excitatory inputs from both the BAS and the BIS. The BAS initiates behavior in response to conditioned stimuli for reward (approach) or for relieving nonpunishment (active avoidance). The BIS, which is viewed as an anxiety system, inhibits behavior in response to cues for punishment (passive avoidance) or frustrative nonreward (extinction), and its activity is decreased by the anti-anxiety drugs (alcohol, barbiturates, minor tranquilizers). Thus, the BIS is an arousal system which inhibits rather than energizes behavior. A review of the literature suggests that heart rate (HR) is strongly associated with activity of the BAS. This interpretation subsumes the previous findings of cardiac-somatic coupling, incentive effects on HR, and increased HR in connection with active coping in the face of threat. Electrodermal activity (EDA), on the other hand, increases when there is an activation of the BIS. A consideration of these differing effects on HR and EDA permits a specification of conditions in which these two measures will or will not show directional fractionation. With this theoretical model it is possible to relate the clinical features of psychopathy to the psychophysiological data with the single assumption that primary psychopaths have a deficient BIS. As a result, they show normal approach, active avoidance, and HR, but they suffer from poor passive avoidance and extinction with reduced EDA in response to threatening stimuli.

[1]  R. Edelberg,et al.  BIOPOTENTIALS FROM THE SKIN SURFACE: THE HYDRATION EFFECT * , 1968, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[2]  A. Ohman,et al.  Electrodermal responses and subjective estimates of UCS probability in a long interstimulus interval conditioning paradigm. , 1976, Psychophysiology.

[3]  V. Houser The Effects of Drugs on Behavior Controlled by Aversive Stimuli , 1978 .

[4]  S. Mangan,et al.  Electrodermal indices of stress and cognition: possible hydration artifacts. , 1979, Psychophysiology.

[5]  D. Lykken,et al.  Psychology and the lie detector industry. , 1974, The American psychologist.

[6]  E. Katkin,et al.  CHAPTER 7 – Systematic Desensitization , 1973 .

[7]  F. Graham Chapter 5 – Habituation and Dishabituation of Responses1 Innervated by the Autonomic Nervous System , 1973 .

[8]  R. Elliott Heart rate, activity, and activation in rats. , 1975 .

[9]  David T. Lykken,et al.  The validity of the guilty knowledge technique: The effects of faking. , 1960 .

[10]  P. Wall,et al.  Pain mechanisms: a new theory. , 1965, Science.

[11]  P. Niemelä Electrodermal responses as a function of quantified threat. , 1969, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[12]  J. Lanzetta,et al.  Effects of the expression of pain on autonomic and pain tolerance responses to subject-controlled pain. , 1977, Psychophysiology.

[13]  Heart rate as a function of deprivation and age in rats. , 1970, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[14]  A. Routtenberg The two-arousal hypothesis: reticular formation and limbic system. , 1968, Psychological review.

[15]  A. Ohman,et al.  Phobias and preparedness: phobic versus neutral pictures as conditioned stimuli for human autonomic responses. , 1975, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[16]  S. Epstein,et al.  Theory and experiment on the measurement of approach-avoidance conflict. , 1962, Journal of abnormal and social psychology.

[17]  J. Hokanson,et al.  Food deprivation, performance, and heart rate in the rat. , 1968, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[18]  G. Bignami Effects of Neuroleptics, Ethanol, Hypnotic-Sedatives, Tranquilizers, Narcotics, and Minor Stimulants in Aversive Paradigms , 1978 .

[19]  R. Ducharme [Physical activity and deactivation: decrease of the cardiac rhythm during instrumental activity]. , 1966, Canadian journal of psychology.

[20]  E. Katkin RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN MANIFEST ANXIETY AND TWO INDICES OF AUTONOMIC RESPONSE TO STRESS. , 1965, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[21]  David C. Raskin,et al.  CHAPTER 2 – Attention and Arousal , 1973 .

[22]  R. Elliott,et al.  Differences in the motivational significance of heart rate and palmar conductance: two tests of a hypothesis. , 1970, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[23]  C. Conners,et al.  THE EFFECTS OF METHYLPHENIDATE ON SYMPTOMATOLOGY AND LEARNING IN DISTURBED CHILDREN. , 1963, The American journal of psychiatry.

[24]  P. Obrist,et al.  Presidential Address, 1975. The cardiovascular-behavioral interaction--as it appears today. , 1976, Psychophysiology.

[25]  J. Stern,et al.  GENERALIZABILITY OF HEART RATE AS A MEASURE OF DRIVE STATE. , 1964, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[26]  J. Millichap DRUGS IN MANAGEMENT OF MINIMAL BRAIN DYSFUNCTION , 1973, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[27]  R. E. Schneider,et al.  A convenient, non-hydrating electrolyte medium for the measurement of electrodermal activity. , 1978, Psychophysiology.

[28]  L. Lipsitt,et al.  Neonatal nutritive sucking: effects of taste stimulation upon sucking rhythm and heart rate. , 1976, Child development.

[29]  A. Ohman,et al.  The premise of equipotentiality in human classical conditioning: conditioned electrodermal responses to potentially phobic stimuli. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[30]  Camil Bouchard,et al.  Heart rate regulation with success and failure signals. , 1976, Psychophysiology.

[31]  B. Skinner,et al.  Some quantitative properties of anxiety , 1941 .

[32]  D G Kilpatrick,et al.  Differential responsiveness of two electrodermal indices to psychological stress and performance of a complex cognitive task. , 1972, Psychophysiology.

[33]  R. Elliott Tonic heart rate: experiments on the effects of collative variables lead to a hypothesis about its motivational significance. , 1969, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[34]  J. Millichap,et al.  Treatment of "minimal brain dysfunction" syndromes. Selection of drugs for children with hyperactivity and learning disabilities. , 1967, The Pediatric clinics of North America.

[35]  S. Hanson,et al.  Arousal: its genesis and manifestation as response rate. , 1978, Psychological review.

[36]  D. Lykken The GSR in the detection of guilt. , 1959 .

[37]  L. Lipsitt,et al.  The stability and interrelationships of newborn sucking and heart rate. , 1976, Developmental psychobiology.

[38]  L. H. Miller,et al.  Replicability of two GSR indices as a function of stress and cognitive activity. , 1965, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[39]  George Mandler,et al.  Chapter 10 – HELPLESSNESS: THEORY AND RESEARCH IN ANXIETY , 1972 .

[40]  R. E. Schneider,et al.  Effects of epidermal hydration on skin conductance responses and levels. , 1974, Biological psychology.

[41]  J. A. Vaughan,et al.  Human eccrine sweat gland activity and palmar electrical skin resistance. , 1965, Journal of applied physiology.

[42]  G. Wilson GSR Responses to Fear-Related Stimuli , 1967, Perceptual and motor skills.

[43]  J. Mørland,et al.  Effects of diazepam and ethanol on heart rate and galvanic skin responses. , 2009, Acta pharmacologica et toxicologica.

[44]  C. Crook Neonatal sucking: effects of quantity of the response-contingent fluid upon sucking rhythm and heart rate. , 1976, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[45]  A. Ohman,et al.  One-trial learning and superior resistance to extinction of autonomic responses conditioned to potentially phobic stimuli. , 1975, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[46]  E. Duffy,et al.  The psychological significance of the concept of arousal or activation. , 1957, Psychological review.

[47]  A. Routtenberg The reward system of the brain. , 1978, Scientific American.

[48]  D. Hebb Drives and the C.N.S. (conceptual nervous system). , 1955, Psychological review.

[49]  A. Amsel Frustrative nonreward in partial reinforcement and discrimination learning: some recent history and a theoretical extension. , 1962, Psychological review.

[50]  E. Duffy,et al.  The concept of energy mobilization. , 1951, Psychological review.

[51]  T. Adams,et al.  Characteristics of eccrine sweat gland activity in the footpad of the cat. , 1966, Journal of applied physiology.

[52]  R. E. Schneider,et al.  Electrolyte medium effects on measurements of palmar skin potential. , 1978, Psychophysiology.

[53]  J. Lanzetta,et al.  Effects of nonverbal dissimulation on emotional experience and autonomic arousal. , 1976, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[54]  A. S. Bernstein,et al.  The phasic electrodermal response as a differentiated complex reflecting stimulus significance. , 1975, Psychophysiology.

[55]  A. Amsel,et al.  Motivational properties of frustration. I. Effect on a running response of the addition of frustration to the motivational complex. , 1952, Journal of experimental psychology.

[56]  J. Pearce,et al.  Inhibitory interactions between appetitive and aversive stimuli. , 1977 .

[57]  H. D. Beach,et al.  The effects of ethanol on the acquistion of a conditioned GSR. , 1968, Quarterly journal of studies on alcohol.

[58]  A. Amsel The role of frustrative nonreward in noncontinuous reward situations. , 1958, Psychological bulletin.

[59]  William F. Prokasy,et al.  CHAPTER 3 – Classical Conditioning , 1973 .

[60]  F. J. Schmauk Punishment, arousal, and avoidance learning in sociopaths. , 1970, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[61]  J. Geer Fear and autonomic arousal. , 1966, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[62]  B Tursky,et al.  Reliability and interjudgment predictability of subjective judgments of electrocutaneous stimulation. , 1972, Psychophysiology.

[63]  R B MALMO,et al.  Activation: a neuropsychological dimension. , 1959, Rassegna giuliana di medicina.

[64]  G. Lienert,et al.  The Effects of Meprobamate and Alcohol on Galvanic Skin Response , 1959 .

[65]  P. Obrist,et al.  Cardiac deceleration and reaction time: an evaluation of two hypotheses. , 1970, Psychophysiology.

[66]  D. Belanger,et al.  Effects of water deprivation upon heart rate and instrumental activity in the rat. , 1962, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[67]  L. Cook,et al.  Behavioral Pharmacology of Antianxiety Drugs , 1978 .

[68]  H. Mccurdy,et al.  Consciousness and the galvanometer. , 1950, Psychological review.

[69]  J. Gray,et al.  The psychophysiological basis of introversion-extraversion. , 1970, Behaviour research and therapy.

[70]  R. Malmo,et al.  Electrophysiological concomitants of simple operant conditioning in the rat , 1967 .

[71]  J. Lacey Psychophysiological approaches to the evaluation of psychotherapeutic process and outcome. , 1959 .

[72]  H. Fitzgerald,et al.  Stimulus specificity of electrodermal recovery time: an examination and reinterpretation of the evidence. , 1975, Psychophysiology.

[73]  R. A. Webb,et al.  The cardiac-somatic relationship: some reformulations. , 1970, Psychophysiology.

[74]  R K Clifton,et al.  Heart-rate change as a component of the orienting response. , 1966, Psychological bulletin.

[75]  R. Solomon,et al.  An opponent-process theory of motivation. I. Temporal dynamics of affect. , 1974, Psychological review.

[76]  W. M. Waid Skin conductance response to punishment as a predictor and correlate of learning to avoid two classes of punishment. , 1976, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[77]  D. Lykken,et al.  A study of anxiety in the sociopathic personality. , 1957, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[78]  S. Epstein,et al.  Availability of an avoidance response as related to autonomic arousal. , 1976, Journal of abnormal psychology.