Speed of response initiation in a time-to-contact discrimination task reflects the use of η
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Simon K. Rushton,et al. Weighted combination of size and disparity: a computational model for timing a ball catch , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[2] D. Regan,et al. Visual processing of looming and time to contact throughout the visual field , 1995, Vision Research.
[3] D. Pins,et al. On the relation between stimulus intensity and processing time: Piéron’s law and choice reaction time , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.
[4] J T Todd,et al. Visual information about moving objects. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[5] G. Laurent,et al. Collision-avoidance: nature's many solutions , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.
[6] Scott M. Dittman,et al. Monocular optical constraints on collision control. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[7] D. Ingle,et al. Brain mechanisms and spatial vision , 1985 .
[8] P R DeLucia,et al. Pictorial and motion-based information for depth perception. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[9] Gaston H. Gonnet,et al. On the LambertW function , 1996, Adv. Comput. Math..
[10] John P. Wann,et al. Anticipating arrival: is the tau margin a specious theory? , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[11] B. Frost,et al. Computation of different optical variables of looming objects in pigeon nucleus rotundus neurons , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.
[12] C Bonnet,et al. The Piéron function in the threshold region , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[13] H. Whiting,et al. Visual occlusion factors in a discrete ball-catching task. , 1974, Journal of motor behavior.
[14] Heiko Hecht,et al. Image Velocity, Not Tau, Explains Arrival-Time Judgments From Global Optical Flow , 1999 .
[15] G. Laurent,et al. Elementary Computation of Object Approach by a Wide-Field Visual Neuron , 1995, Science.
[16] D Regan,et al. Accuracy of estimating time to collision using binocular and monocular information , 1998, Vision Research.
[17] J. Tresilian,et al. Perceptual and cognitive processes in time-to-contact estimation: Analysis of prediction-motion and relative judgment tasks , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.
[18] C Kaernbach,et al. Simple adaptive testing with the weighted up-down method , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.
[19] J. Tresilian. Visually timed action: time-out for ‘tau’? , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[20] Paul R. Schrater,et al. Perceiving visual expansion without optic flow , 2001, Nature.
[21] E Brenner,et al. Is Judging Time-to-Contact Based on ‘Tau’? , 1996, Perception.
[22] D. Regan,et al. Dissociation of discrimination thresholds for time to contact and for rate of angular expansion , 1993, Vision Research.
[23] Michael F. Land. Brain Mechanisms and Spatial Vision, David J. Ingle, Marc Jeannerod, David N. Lee (Eds.). Martinus Nijhoff, Dordrecht, The Netherlands (1985), viii , 1985 .
[24] R Gray,et al. Attentional modulation of motion-in-depth processing , 2000, Vision Research.
[25] Vision Research , 1961, Nature.
[26] Patricia R. Delucia,et al. Size-arrival effects: The potential roles of conflicts between monocular and binocular time-to-contact information, and of computer aliasing , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.
[27] J. Schall,et al. Neural Control of Voluntary Movement Initiation , 1996, Science.