A Non-Lévy Random Walk in Chacma Baboons: What Does It Mean?

The Lévy walk is found from amoebas to humans and has been described as the optimal strategy for food research. Recent results, however, have generated controversy about this conclusion since animals also display alternatives to the Lévy walk such as the Brownian walk or mental maps and because movement patterns found in some species only seem to depend on food patches distribution. Here I show that movement patterns of chacma baboons do not follow a Lévy walk but a Brownian process. Moreover this Brownian walk is not the main process responsible for movement patterns of baboons. Findings about their speed and trajectories show that baboons use metal maps and memory to find resources. Thus the Brownian process found in this species appears to be more dependent on the environment or might be an alternative when known food patches are depleted and when animals have to find new resources.

[1]  H. Larralde,et al.  Lévy walk patterns in the foraging movements of spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) , 2003, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[2]  P. Garber,et al.  Experimental field study of spatial memory and learning in wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus). , 1997, Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology.

[3]  Richard Byrne,et al.  What wild primates know about resources: opening up the black box , 2007, Animal Cognition.

[4]  H. Fritz,et al.  Scale–dependent hierarchical adjustments of movement patterns in a long–range foraging seabird , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[5]  H. Kummer,et al.  Primate Societies: Group Techniques of Ecological Adaptation , 1971 .

[6]  C. Menzel Cognitive aspects of foraging in Japanese monkeys , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[7]  James F. Brown XXIV. On some salts and products of decomposition of pyromeconic acid , 1852 .

[8]  Matt Grove,et al.  Ranging patterns of hamadryas baboons: random walk analyses , 2010, Animal Behaviour.

[9]  D. Macdonald,et al.  Scale‐free dynamics in the movement patterns of jackals , 2002 .

[10]  H. L. Dryden,et al.  Investigations on the Theory of the Brownian Movement , 1957 .

[11]  D. Boyera,et al.  Modeling the searching behavior of social monkeys , 2004 .

[12]  G. Gerold,et al.  "Land Use, Nature Conservation and the Stability of Rainforest Margins in Southeast Asia" , 2010 .

[13]  Michael F. Shlesinger,et al.  Strange kinetics , 1993, Nature.

[14]  H. Kummer,et al.  Social Organization of Hamadryas Baboons: A Field Study , 1967 .

[15]  R. Brown XXVII. A brief account of microscopical observations made in the months of June, July and August 1827, on the particles contained in the pollen of plants; and on the general existence of active molecules in organic and inorganic bodies , 1828 .

[16]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Primate Cognition , 2010, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[17]  H. Stanley,et al.  Optimizing the success of random searches , 1999, Nature.

[18]  H. Kummer,et al.  Social Organization Of Hamadryas Baboons , 1968 .

[19]  G. Viswanathan,et al.  Lévy flights and superdiffusion in the context of biological encounters and random searches , 2008 .

[20]  E. Riley Ranging patterns and habitat use of Sulawesi Tonkean macaques (Macaca tonkeana) in a human‐modified habitat , 2008, American journal of primatology.

[21]  K. Zuberbühler,et al.  Foraging cognition in nonhuman primates , 2010 .

[22]  H. Sigg,et al.  Home range and daily march in a Hamadryas baboon troop. , 1981, Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology.

[23]  Bruce Page,et al.  SHORT-DURATION DAYTIME MOVEMENTS OF A COW HERD OF AFRICAN ELEPHANTS , 2007 .

[24]  Nicolas E. Humphries,et al.  Environmental context explains Lévy and Brownian movement patterns of marine predators , 2010, Nature.

[25]  Simon Benhamou,et al.  How many animals really do the Lévy walk? , 2008, Ecology.

[26]  C. Menzel,et al.  Structure‐guided foraging in long‐tailed macaques , 1996, American journal of primatology.

[27]  P. A. Prince,et al.  Lévy flight search patterns of wandering albatrosses , 1996, Nature.

[28]  A. Mardiastuti,et al.  Home Range, Diet and Behaviour of the Tonkean Macaque (Macaca tonkeana) in Lore Lindu National Park, Sulawesi , 2004 .

[29]  Paul R Moorcroft,et al.  Stochastic modelling of animal movement , 2010, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[30]  Richard W. Byrne,et al.  Travel routes and planning of visits to out-of-sight resources in wild chacma baboons, Papio ursinus , 2007, Animal Behaviour.

[31]  P. Garber,et al.  Adaptive Radiations of Neotropical Primates , 1996, Springer US.

[32]  Germinal Cocho,et al.  Scale-free foraging by primates emerges from their interaction with a complex environment , 2006, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[33]  D. Wilkie,et al.  Yellow-nosed monkeys' (Cercopithecus ascanius whitesidei) spatial memory in a simulated foraging environment. , 1990 .

[34]  F Bartumeus,et al.  Optimizing the encounter rate in biological interactions: Lévy versus Brownian strategies. , 2002, Physical review letters.

[35]  John G. Robinson On the Move. How and Why Animals Travel in Groups , 2000, Animal Behaviour.

[36]  M. Levandowsky,et al.  Random movements of soil amebas , 1997 .

[37]  C. Janson TOWARD AN EXPERIMENTAL SOCIOECOLOGY OF PRIMATES , 1996 .