Involuntary orienting of attention to nociceptive events: neural and behavioral signatures.

Pain can involuntarily capture attention and disrupt pain-unrelated cognitive activities. The brain mechanisms of these effects were explored by laser- and visual-evoked potentials. Consecutive nociceptive laser stimuli and visual stimuli were delivered in pairs. Subjects were instructed to ignore nociceptive stimuli while performing a task on visual targets. Because involuntary attention is particularly sensitive to novelty, in some trials (17%), unexpected laser stimuli were delivered on a different hand area (location-deviant) relative to the more frequent standard laser stimuli. Compared with frequent standard laser stimuli, deviant stimuli enhanced all nociceptive-evoked brain potentials (laser N1, N2, P2a, P2b). Deviant laser stimuli also decreased the amplitude of late latency-evoked responses (visual N2-P3) to the subsequent visual targets and delayed reaction times to them. The data confirm that nociceptive processing competes with pain-unrelated cognitive activities for attentional resources and that concomitant nociceptive events affect behavior by depressing attention allocation to ongoing cognitive processing. The laser-evoked potential magnitude reflected the engagement of attention to the novel nociceptive stimuli. We conclude that the laser-evoked potentials index the activity of a neural system involved in the detection of novel salient stimuli in order to focus attention and prioritize action to potentially damaging dangers.

[1]  D. Broadbent Perception and communication , 1958 .

[2]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Attention and Effort , 1973 .

[3]  E. N. Sokolov,et al.  Neuronal mechanisms of the orienting reflex , 1975 .

[4]  N. Squires,et al.  Two varieties of long-latency positive waves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in man. , 1975, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[5]  E. Courchesne,et al.  Stimulus novelty, task relevance and the visual evoked potential in man. , 1975, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[6]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Stimulus deviance and evoked potentials , 1982, Biological Psychology.

[7]  M. Posner,et al.  Components of visual orienting , 1984 .

[8]  M. Bushnell,et al.  Attentional influences on noxious and innocuous cutaneous heat detection in humans and monkeys , 1985 .

[9]  C. C. Wood,et al.  Scalp distributions of event-related potentials: an ambiguity associated with analysis of variance models. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[10]  T. Picton,et al.  The N1 wave of the human electric and magnetic response to sound: a review and an analysis of the component structure. , 1987, Psychophysiology.

[11]  M. Bushnell,et al.  Effects of attention on the intensity and unpleasantness of thermal pain , 1989, Pain.

[12]  K. Reinikainen,et al.  Do event-related potentials reveal the mechanism of the auditory sensory memory in the human brain? , 1989, Neuroscience Letters.

[13]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  J. Rosenfeld,et al.  Ongoing pain as a mental workload indexed by P300 depression: discrimination of real and feigned pain conditions. , 1991, Psychophysiology.

[15]  D. Woods,et al.  Auditory selective attention in middle-aged and elderly subjects: an event-related brain potential study. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[16]  J. Rosenfeld,et al.  Ongoing ischemic pain as a workload indexed by P3 amplitude and latency in real-versus feigned-pain conditions. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[17]  R. Treede,et al.  Topography of middle-latency somatosensory evoked potentials following painful laser stimuli and non-painful electrical stimuli. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[18]  H. Pashler Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[19]  S. Yantis,et al.  Stimulus-driven attentional capture: evidence from equiluminant visual objects. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  J. Theeuwes Stimulus-driven capture and attentional set: selective search for color and visual abrupt onsets. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  G. Crombez,et al.  Sensory and temporal information about impending pain: the influence of predictability on pain. , 1994, Behaviour research and therapy.

[22]  G. Crombez,et al.  The disruptive nature of pain: an experimental investigation. , 1996, Behaviour research and therapy.

[23]  Erich Schrger,et al.  A Neural Mechanism for Involuntary Attention Shifts to Changes in Auditory Stimulation , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[24]  K Alho,et al.  Effects of involuntary auditory attention on visual task performance and brain activity , 1997, Neuroreport.

[25]  Angela D. Friederici,et al.  Semantic aspects of novelty detection in humans , 1997, Neuroscience Letters.

[26]  C D Frith,et al.  Modulating irrelevant motion perception by varying attentional load in an unrelated task. , 1997, Science.

[27]  Frank Baeyens,et al.  Habituation and the interference of pain with task performance , 1997, Pain.

[28]  G. Berns,et al.  Brain regions responsive to novelty in the absence of awareness. , 1997, Science.

[29]  B Bromm,et al.  Event-related potential correlates of interference between cognitive performance and tonic experimental pain. , 1997, Psychophysiology.

[30]  R. Knight,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Involuntary Attention to Acoustic Novelty and Change , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[31]  E. Schröger,et al.  Behavioral and electrophysiological effects of task-irrelevant sound change: a new distraction paradigm. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[32]  J. Polich,et al.  Stimulus context determines P3a and P3b. , 1998, Psychophysiology.

[33]  E. Vogel,et al.  Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[34]  Frank Baeyens,et al.  When somatic information threatens, catastrophic thinking enhances attentional interference , 1998, Pain.

[35]  G. Crombez,et al.  Attentional disruption is enhanced by the threat of pain. , 1998, Behaviour research and therapy.

[36]  G. Crombez,et al.  Pain demands attention: a cognitive-affective model of the interruptive function of pain. , 1999, Psychological bulletin.

[37]  M. Bushnell,et al.  Pain perception: is there a role for primary somatosensory cortex? , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[38]  T. Picton,et al.  Mismatch Negativity: Different Water in the Same River , 2000, Audiology and Neurotology.

[39]  J. Downar,et al.  A multimodal cortical network for the detection of changes in the sensory environment , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[40]  R. Benson,et al.  Responses to rare visual target and distractor stimuli using event-related fMRI. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  S. Stone-Elander,et al.  Pain-related cerebral activation is altered by a distracting cognitive task , 2000, Pain.

[42]  D. Simons Attentional capture and inattentional blindness , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[43]  C. Frith,et al.  The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention , 2001, Science.

[44]  John Polich,et al.  P3a from a passive visual stimulus task , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[45]  C. Escera,et al.  Electrical responses reveal the temporal dynamics of brain events during involuntary attention switching , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[46]  D. Friedman,et al.  The novelty P3: an event-related brain potential (ERP) sign of the brain's evaluation of novelty , 2001, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[47]  C. Escera,et al.  Activation of brain mechanisms of attention switching as a function of auditory frequency change , 2001, Neuroreport.

[48]  A. K. Jones,et al.  Source localisation of 62-electrode human laser pain evoked potential data using a realistic head model. , 2001, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[49]  G. Mangun,et al.  Perceptual Load and Visuocortical Processing: Event-Related Potentials Reveal Sensory-Level Selection , 2001, Psychological science.

[50]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[51]  V. Legrain,et al.  Attentional modulation of the nociceptive processing into the human brain: selective spatial attention, probability of stimulus occurrence, and target detection effects on laser evoked potentials , 2002, PAIN.

[52]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  A review of attentional capture: On its automaticity and sensitivity to endogenous control. , 2002 .

[53]  Roger Zimmermann,et al.  Attentional modulation of human pain processing in the secondary somatosensory cortex: a magnetoencephalographic study , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.

[54]  Charles Spence,et al.  Selective attention to pain: a psychophysical investigation , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[55]  Carles Escera,et al.  Attention capture by auditory significant stimuli: semantic analysis follows attention switching , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[56]  Jonathan Downar,et al.  Neural correlates of the prolonged salience of painful stimulation , 2003, NeuroImage.

[57]  V. Jousmäki,et al.  Effects of Interstimulus Interval on Cortical Responses to Painful Laser Stimulation , 2003, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[58]  Carles Escera,et al.  Spatiotemporal dynamics of the auditory novelty-P3 event-related brain potential. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[59]  Léon Plaghki,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of attentional orientation in humans to strong intensity deviant nociceptive stimuli, inside and outside the focus of spatial attention , 2003, Neuroscience Letters.

[60]  M. Frot,et al.  Brain generators of laser-evoked potentials: from dipoles to functional significance , 2003, Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology.

[61]  Léon Plaghki,et al.  Nociceptive processing in the human brain of infrequent task-relevant and task-irrelevant noxious stimuli. A study with event-related potentials evoked by CO2 laser radiant heat stimuli , 2003, Pain.

[62]  C. Escera,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence of abnormal activation of the cerebral network of involuntary attention in alcoholism , 2003, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[63]  Risto Näätänen,et al.  Effects of auditory distraction on electrophysiological brain activity and performance in children aged 8-13 years. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[64]  Luis Garcia-Larrea,et al.  Somatosensory volleys and cortical evoked potentials: ‘First come, first served’? , 2004, Pain.

[65]  Y. Sarne,et al.  Correlation of subjective pain experience with cerebral evoked responses to noxious thermal stimulations , 1978, Experimental Brain Research.

[66]  K. Crowley,et al.  A review of the evidence for P2 being an independent component process: age, sleep and modality , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[67]  Rolf-Detlef Treede,et al.  Attention to pain is processed at multiple cortical sites in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[68]  A. Carmon,et al.  Evoked cerebral responses to noxious thermal stimuli in humans , 1976, Experimental Brain Research.

[69]  G. Crombez,et al.  Hypervigilance to pain: An experimental and clinical analysis , 2005, Pain.

[70]  André Mouraux,et al.  EEG and laser stimulation as tools for pain research. , 2005, Current opinion in investigational drugs.

[71]  I. Tracey Nociceptive processing in the human brain , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[72]  Martin J. McKeown,et al.  Acute and persistent pain modulation of attention-related anterior cingulate fMRI activations , 2005, Pain.

[73]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence of enhanced distractibility in ADHD children , 2005, Neuroscience Letters.

[74]  N. Lavie Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[75]  V. Legrain,et al.  Involuntary orientation of attention to unattended deviant nociceptive stimuli is modulated by concomitant visual task difficulty. Evidence from laser evoked potentials , 2005, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[76]  E. Schröger,et al.  Two separate mechanisms underlie auditory change detection and involuntary control of attention , 2006, Brain Research.

[77]  L. Vancleef,et al.  Pain catastrophizing, but not injury/illness sensitivity or anxiety sensitivity, enhances attentional interference by pain. , 2006, The journal of pain : official journal of the American Pain Society.

[78]  L. Vancleef,et al.  The interruptive effect of pain on attention. , 2006, The journal of pain : official journal of the American Pain Society.

[79]  J. Kenemans,et al.  Pain and attention: attentional disruption or distraction? , 2006, The journal of pain : official journal of the American Pain Society.

[80]  Christian Büchel,et al.  fMRI Reveals How Pain Modulates Visual Object Processing in the Ventral Visual Stream , 2007, Neuron.

[81]  R. Näätänen,et al.  The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: A review , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[82]  K. Davis,et al.  Pain enhances functional connectivity of a brain network evoked by performance of a cognitive task. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[83]  C. Escera,et al.  Role of Mismatch Negativity and Novelty-P3 in Involuntary Auditory Attention , 2007 .

[84]  Karen D Davis,et al.  Interactions of pain intensity and cognitive load: the brain stays on task. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[85]  V. Legrain La modulation de la douleur par l’attention. Les apports de la neurophysiologie , 2008 .

[86]  Phillip J. Holcomb,et al.  To Ignore or Explore: Top-Down Modulation of Novelty Processing , 2008, J. Cogn. Neurosci..

[87]  S. Yantis The Neural Basis of Selective Attention , 2008, Current directions in psychological science.

[88]  V. Legrain,et al.  Laser evoked responses to painful stimulation persist during sleep and predict subsequent arousals , 2008, PAIN®.

[89]  Valéry Legrain,et al.  Evoked potentials to nociceptive stimuli delivered by CO2 or Nd:YAP lasers , 2008, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[90]  A. Mouraux,et al.  Determinants of laser-evoked EEG responses: pain perception or stimulus saliency? , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[91]  François Mauguière,et al.  Parallel Processing of Nociceptive A-δ Inputs in SII and Midcingulate Cortex in Humans , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[92]  A. Mouraux,et al.  A review of the evidence against the “first come first served” hypothesis. Comment on Truini et al. [Pain 2007;131:43–7] , 2008, PAIN.

[93]  Carles Escera,et al.  When Loading Working Memory Reduces Distraction: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence from an Auditory-Visual Distraction Paradigm , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[94]  P012 Evoked potentials in response to painful stimuli delivered by CO2 or Nd:YAP Laser , 2008, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[95]  A. Mouraux,et al.  Nociceptive laser-evoked brain potentials do not reflect nociceptive-specific neural activity. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[96]  K. Davis,et al.  A neurocognitive model of attention to pain: Behavioral and neuroimaging evidence , 2009, PAIN®.

[97]  G. Crombez,et al.  Keeping pain in mind: A motivational account of attention to pain , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.