Tracking behavioral and neural fluctuations during sustained attention: A robust replication and extension

&NA; Novel paradigms have allowed for more precise measurements of sustained attention ability and fluctuations in sustained attention over time, as well as the neural basis of fluctuations and lapses in performance. However, in recent years, concerns have arisen over the replicability of neuroimaging studies and psychology more broadly, particularly given the typically small sample sizes. One recently developed paradigm, the gradual‐onset continuous performance task (gradCPT) has been validated behaviorally in large samples of participants. Yet neuroimaging studies investigating the neural basis of performance on this task have only been collected in small samples. The present study completed both a robust replication of the original neuroimaging findings and extended previous results from the gradCPT task using a large sample of 140 Veteran participants. Results replicate findings that fluctuations in attentional stability are tracked over time by BOLD activity in task positive (e.g., dorsal and ventral attention networks) and task negative (e.g., default network) regions. Extending prior results, we relate this coupling between attentional stability and on‐going brain activity to overall sustained attention ability and demonstrate that this coupling strength, along with across‐network coupling, could be used to predict individual differences in performance. Additionally, the results extend previous findings by demonstrating that temporal dynamics across the default and dorsal attention networks are associated with lapse‐likelihood on subsequent trials. This study demonstrates the reliability of the gradCPT, and underscores the utility of this paradigm in understanding attentional fluctuations, as well as individual variation and deficits in sustained attention. HighlightsA robust fMRI replication with N=140 of the gradCPT sustained attention paradigm.Neural coupling with fluctuations in reaction time variability predicts performance.Neural trajectories prior to target onset vary with likelihood of attention lapses.

[1]  M. Esterman,et al.  Spontaneous default network activity reflects behavioral variability independent of mind-wandering , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[2]  N. Yeung,et al.  The roles of cortical oscillations in sustained attention , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  Benjamin J. Tamber-Rosenau,et al.  Avoiding non-independence in fMRI data analysis: Leave one subject out , 2010, NeuroImage.

[4]  E. Stein,et al.  Multiple Neuronal Networks Mediate Sustained Attention , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[5]  Michael Esterman,et al.  Intrinsic Fluctuations in Sustained Attention and Distractor Processing , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  Michael Esterman,et al.  Posttraumatic Psychological Symptoms are Associated with Reduced Inhibitory Control, not General Executive Dysfunction , 2015, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[7]  H. Silver,et al.  Evidence for sustained attention and working memory in schizophrenia sharing a common mechanism. , 2005, The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[8]  Francesca C. Fortenbaugh,et al.  Sustained Attention Across the Life Span in a Sample of 10,000 , 2015, Psychological science.

[9]  W. Milberg,et al.  A methodology for assessing deployment trauma and its consequences in OEF/OIF/OND veterans: The TRACTS longitudinal prospective cohort study , 2017, International journal of methods in psychiatric research.

[10]  Michael J. Hove,et al.  Dynamic Brain Network Correlates of Spontaneous Fluctuations in Attention , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[11]  Paul A. Taylor,et al.  FMRI Clustering in AFNI: False-Positive Rates Redux , 2017, Brain Connect..

[12]  P MALABIA,et al.  [Dynamic brain]. , 1956, Medicina espanola.

[13]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[14]  William P. Milberg,et al.  Deployment-related psychiatric and behavioral conditions and their association with functional disability in OEF/OIF/OND veterans. , 2015, Journal of traumatic stress.

[15]  Joel S. Warm,et al.  The Effects of Signal Salience and Caffeine on Performance, Workload, and Stress in an Abbreviated Vigilance Task , 2000, Hum. Factors.

[16]  Guanyu Liu,et al.  Modulating Reward Induces Differential Neurocognitive Approaches to Sustained Attention , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  R. Shah,et al.  Sleepiness, parkinsonian features and sustained attention in mild Alzheimer's disease. , 2012, Age and ageing.

[18]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Scanning the horizon: towards transparent and reproducible neuroimaging research , 2016, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[19]  Nikos Makris,et al.  Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[20]  M. Rosenberg,et al.  In the zone or zoning out? Tracking behavioral and neural fluctuations during sustained attention. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[21]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[22]  Viviana Betti,et al.  Cortical cores in network dynamics , 2018, NeuroImage.

[23]  S. Petersen,et al.  A dual-networks architecture of top-down control , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  N. Mackworth The Breakdown of Vigilance during Prolonged Visual Search 1 , 1948 .

[25]  R. Barkley Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[26]  D. Heeger,et al.  Topographic maps of visual spatial attention in human parietal cortex. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Wandering Minds: The Default Network and Stimulus-Independent Thought , 2007, Science.

[28]  F. Lévy,et al.  The development of sustained attention (vigilance) and inhibition in children: some normative data. , 1980, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[29]  Nicholas A. Steinmetz,et al.  Frontal eye field , 2012, Scholarpedia.

[30]  Michael Esterman,et al.  Sustaining visual attention in the face of distraction: a novel gradual-onset continuous performance task , 2013, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[31]  Francesca C. Fortenbaugh,et al.  Interpersonal early‐life trauma alters amygdala connectivity and sustained attention performance , 2017, Brain and behavior.

[32]  Anne Bonnefond,et al.  Investigating sustained attention ability in the elderly by using two different approaches: inhibiting ongoing behavior versus responding on rare occasions. , 2014, Acta psychologica.

[33]  Zachary C. Irving,et al.  Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: a dynamic framework , 2016, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[34]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience , 2013, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[35]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.

[36]  Jonathan S. A. Carriere,et al.  Age trends for failures of sustained attention. , 2010, Psychology and aging.

[37]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  A purely confirmatory replication study of structural brain-behavior correlations , 2015, Cortex.

[38]  Diane Swick,et al.  Ten years of inhibition revisited , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[39]  S. Eickhoff,et al.  Sustaining attention to simple tasks: a meta-analytic review of the neural mechanisms of vigilant attention. , 2013, Psychological bulletin.

[40]  Hillary D. Schwarb,et al.  Short‐time windows of correlation between large‐scale functional brain networks predict vigilance intraindividually and interindividually , 2013, Human brain mapping.

[41]  David K. Menon,et al.  Default mode contributions to automated information processing , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[42]  P. Bentler,et al.  Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis : Conventional criteria versus new alternatives , 1999 .

[43]  J. Haxby,et al.  Overall vigilance and sustained attention decrements in healthy aging. , 2001, Experimental aging research.

[44]  Lynn C. Robertson,et al.  Changes in the distribution of sustained attention alter the perceived structure of visual space , 2017, Vision Research.

[45]  Francesca C. Fortenbaugh,et al.  Recent theoretical, neural, and clinical advances in sustained attention research , 2017, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[46]  Haakon G. Engen,et al.  Shaped by the Past: The Default Mode Network Supports Cognition that Is Independent of Immediate Perceptual Input , 2015, PloS one.

[47]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[48]  I. Robertson,et al.  `Oops!': Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[49]  Yehoshua Tsal,et al.  Conjunctive Continuous Performance Task (CCPT)—A pure measure of sustained attention , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[50]  Bérengère Staub,et al.  Sustained attention in the elderly: What do we know and what does it tell us about cognitive aging? , 2013, Ageing Research Reviews.

[51]  Marisa O. Hollinshead,et al.  The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[52]  J. Wilmer,et al.  Gender Differences in Sustained Attentional Control Relate to Gender Inequality across Countries , 2016, PloS one.

[53]  M. Sarter,et al.  The cognitive neuroscience of sustained attention: where top-down meets bottom-up , 2001, Brain Research Reviews.

[54]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Functional Characterization of the Cingulo-Opercular Network in the Maintenance of Tonic Alertness. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[55]  M. Esterman,et al.  Anticipation of Monetary Reward Can Attenuate the Vigilance Decrement , 2016, PloS one.

[56]  J. Pillai Functional Connectivity. , 2017, Neuroimaging clinics of North America.

[57]  Guanyu Liu,et al.  Reward reveals dissociable aspects of sustained attention. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[58]  M. Chun,et al.  A neuromarker of sustained attention from whole-brain functional connectivity , 2015, Nature Neuroscience.

[59]  J. Redman,et al.  The Effect of Traumatic Brain Injury on the Timing of Sleep , 2005, Chronobiology international.

[60]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[61]  Francesca C. Fortenbaugh,et al.  Time-of-day variation in sustained attentional control , 2017, Chronobiology international.

[62]  J. H. Steiger Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix. , 1980 .

[63]  Guanyu Liu,et al.  Frontal eye field involvement in sustaining visual attention: Evidence from transcranial magnetic stimulation , 2015, NeuroImage.

[64]  Marvin M. Chun,et al.  Predicting moment-to-moment attentional state , 2014, NeuroImage.

[65]  James B. Schreiber,et al.  Reporting Structural Equation Modeling and Confirmatory Factor Analysis Results: A Review , 2006 .

[66]  Luke Clark,et al.  Sustained attention deficit in bipolar disorder. , 2002, The British journal of psychiatry : the journal of mental science.

[67]  Sonia J. Bishop,et al.  Unraveling the Anxious Mind: Anxiety, Worry, and Frontal Engagement in Sustained Attention Versus Off-Task Processing , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[68]  M. Hallett,et al.  Involvement of the ipsilateral motor cortex in finger movements of different complexities , 1997, Annals of neurology.

[69]  Diane Swick,et al.  Increased response variability as a marker of executive dysfunction in veterans with post-traumatic stress disorder , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[70]  Mirjam Münch,et al.  Gender and age differences in psychomotor vigilance performance under differential sleep pressure conditions , 2006, Behavioural Brain Research.

[71]  A. Dale,et al.  Cortical Surface-Based Analysis II: Inflation, Flattening, and a Surface-Based Coordinate System , 1999, NeuroImage.

[72]  S. Debener,et al.  Default-mode brain dysfunction in mental disorders: A systematic review , 2009, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[73]  W. Helton,et al.  Working memory load and the vigilance decrement , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[74]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science , 2015, Science.

[75]  David J. Schretlen,et al.  A quantitative review of the effects of traumatic brain injury on cognitive functioning , 2003, International review of psychiatry.

[76]  R. N. Spreng,et al.  The default network and self‐generated thought: component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance , 2014, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[77]  S. Kastner,et al.  Topographic maps in human frontal and parietal cortex , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[78]  M. Nock,et al.  Adolescent nonsuicidal self-injury: Examining the role of child abuse, comorbidity, and disinhibition , 2014, Psychiatry Research.

[79]  Wei Gao,et al.  Frontal parietal control network regulates the anti‐correlated default and dorsal attention networks , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[80]  Joseph M. DeGutis,et al.  Cross-training in hemispatial neglect: Auditory sustained attention training ameliorates visual attention deficits , 2013, Cortex.

[81]  Dustin Scheinost,et al.  Real-time fMRI links subjective experience with brain activity during focused attention , 2013, NeuroImage.

[82]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[83]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[84]  R W Cox,et al.  AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. , 1996, Computers and biomedical research, an international journal.

[85]  Diane Swick,et al.  Impaired Response Inhibition in Veterans with Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder and Mild Traumatic Brain Injury , 2012, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[86]  L. Nyberg,et al.  Age-Related Differences in Dynamic Interactions Among Default Mode, Frontoparietal Control, and Dorsal Attention Networks during Resting-State and Interference Resolution , 2017, Front. Aging Neurosci..

[87]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Closed-loop training of attention with real-time brain imaging , 2015, Nature Neuroscience.

[88]  M. Bellgrove,et al.  Left anterior cingulate activity predicts intra-individual reaction time variability in healthy adults , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[89]  M Mackeben,et al.  The importance of sustained attention for patients with maculopathies , 2000, Vision Research.

[90]  Vinod Menon,et al.  Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[91]  L. Lapointe,et al.  The effect of traumatic brain injury on sustained attention and working memory , 2012, Brain injury.