Effects of anxiety and cognitive load on instrument scanning behavior in a flight simulation

Previous research has rarely examined the combined influence of anxiety and cognitive load on gaze behavior and performance whilst undertaking complex perceptual-motor tasks. In the current study, participants performed an aviation instrument landing task in neutral and anxiety conditions, while performing a low or high cognitive load auditory n-back task. Both self-reported anxiety and heart rate increased from neutral conditions indicating that anxiety was successfully manipulated. Response accuracy and reaction time for the auditory task indicated that cognitive load was also successfully manipulated. Cognitiveloadnegativelyimpactedflightperformance and the frequency of gaze transitions between areas of interest. Performance was maintained in anxious conditions,with a concomitant decrease in n-back reaction time suggesting that this was due to an increase in mental effort. Analyses of individual responses to the anxiety manipulation revealed that changes in anxiety levels from neutral to anxiety conditions were positively correlated with changes in visual scanning entropy, which isa measure of the randomness of gaze behavior, but only when cognitive load was high. This finding lends support for an interactive effect of cognitive anxiety and cognitive load on attentional control.

[1]  R. Oudejans,et al.  Effects of anxiety, a cognitive secondary task, and expertise on gaze behavior and performance in a far aiming task , 2012 .

[2]  C. Grillon,et al.  Describing the interplay between anxiety and cognition: from impaired performance under low cognitive load to reduced anxiety under high load. , 2012, Psychophysiology.

[3]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  Estimating and testing mediation and moderation in within-subject designs. , 2001, Psychological methods.

[4]  Nick Berggren,et al.  Affective attention under cognitive load: reduced emotional biases but emergent anxiety-related costs to inhibitory control , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[5]  John W. Senders,et al.  The Human Operator as a Monitor and Controller of Multidegree of Freedom Systems , 1964 .

[6]  M. Eysenck,et al.  Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. , 2007, Emotion.

[7]  D. Rubin,et al.  Comparing Correlated but Nonoverlapping Correlations , 1996 .

[8]  Christopher D. Wickens,et al.  Pilot Task Management : Testing an Attentional Expected Value Model of Visual Scanning , 2001 .

[9]  Mark R. Wilson,et al.  Individual reactions to stress predict performance during a critical aviation incident , 2015, Anxiety, stress, and coping.

[10]  David McIntyre,et al.  Effects of competitive pressure on expert performance: underlying psychological, physiological, and kinematic mechanisms. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[11]  Mark R. Wilson,et al.  Anxiety, attentional control, and performance impairment in penalty kicks. , 2009, Journal of sport & exercise psychology.

[12]  Andrew R. A. Conway,et al.  Working memory, attention control, and the N-back task: a question of construct validity. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[13]  W. Kirchner Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. , 1958, Journal of experimental psychology.

[14]  Kelly J. Ashford,et al.  Dispositional reinvestment and skill failure in cognitive and motor tasks , 2010 .

[15]  Miwa Hayashi,et al.  Characterizing Scan Patterns in a Spacecraft Cockpit Simulator: Expert Vs. Novice Performance , 2005 .

[16]  Jonathan Allsop,et al.  Flying under pressure: Effects of anxiety on attention and gaze behavior in aviation , 2014 .

[17]  Mark R. Wilson,et al.  State anxiety and visual attention: The role of the quiet eye period in aiming to a far target , 2008, Journal of sports sciences.

[18]  Cody Ding,et al.  Impact of Working Memory Load on Cognitive Control in Trait Anxiety: An ERP Study , 2014, PloS one.

[19]  Roger W. Schvaneveldt,et al.  Visual Misperception in Aviation: Glide Path Performance in a Black Hole Environment , 2008, Hum. Factors.

[20]  J. Causer,et al.  Anxiety, movement kinematics, and visual attention in elite-level performers. , 2011, Emotion.

[21]  N. Berggren,et al.  Attentional control deficits in trait anxiety: Why you see them and why you don’t , 2013, Biological Psychology.

[22]  W. Russell,et al.  Measuring Anxiety in Athletics: The Revised Competitive State Anxiety Inventory–2 , 2003 .

[23]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[24]  A. Williams,et al.  The Effects of Anxiety on Visual Search, Movement Kinematics, and Performance in Table Tennis: A Test of Eysenck and Calvo's Processing Efficiency Theory , 2001 .

[25]  Shama C. Akhtar,et al.  The role of visual occlusion in altitude maintenance during simulated flight. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  Joseph H. Goldberg,et al.  Identifying fixations and saccades in eye-tracking protocols , 2000, ETRA.

[27]  Mark R. Wilson,et al.  The influence of anxiety on visual attentional control in basketball free throw shooting. , 2009, Journal of sport & exercise psychology.

[28]  S R Ellis,et al.  Statistical Dependency in Visual Scanning , 1986, Human factors.

[29]  Mark R. Wilson,et al.  Quiet eye training expedites motor learning and aids performance under heightened anxiety: the roles of response programming and external attention. , 2012, Psychophysiology.