Numerosity heuristic in route choice based on the presence of traffic lights

We analyzed how the presence of traffic lights, as a cause of time loss, is taken into account by drivers in planning their route through urban areas. Our hypothesis was that routes with fewer traffic lights are preferred even if the probability of having to stop at those lights is high and the waiting time at the red light is long. We carried out a questionnaire-based study in which car drivers (n = 194) chose the route they preferred from pairs of hypothetical itineraries. The binary dependent variable was the type of route chosen: either a route containing fewer lights at which being forced to stop was highly probable, or a route containing more lights at which being forced to stop was far less probable. We found that the number of traffic lights was the preferred criterion, and that this preference could sometimes induce non-optimal route choices. Red- and green-light durations were also used as choice criteria. However, manipulation checks showed that participants did not estimate the probability of being forced to wait vs. being able to go through the light. We concluded that they estimated a threshold of acceptable waiting time at red lights depending on the number of traffic lights along the itinerary.

[1]  L. A. Pervin THE NEED TO PREDICT AND CONTROL UNDER CONDITIONS OF THREAT. , 1963, Journal of personality.

[2]  D. Zakay Attention et jugement temporel , 2005 .

[3]  B E Porter,et al.  A nationwide survey of self-reported red light running: measuring prevalence, predictors, and perceived consequences. , 2001, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[4]  Charles Kemp,et al.  How to Grow a Mind: Statistics, Structure, and Abstraction , 2011, Science.

[5]  S. Zeger,et al.  Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models , 1986 .

[6]  Eyal Peer,et al.  Speeding and the time-saving bias: how drivers' estimations of time saved in higher speed affects their choice of speed. , 2010, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[7]  L Hasher,et al.  Automatic processing of fundamental information: the case of frequency of occurrence. , 1984, The American psychologist.

[8]  Carl G. Hoyos Mental load and risk in traffic behaviour , 1988 .

[9]  L. Myaskovsky,et al.  The Easy Path From Many To Much: the Numerosity Heuristic , 1994, Cognitive Psychology.

[10]  C. Gallistel Animal cognition: the representation of space, time and number. , 1989, Annual review of psychology.

[11]  山本 治雄 フランスの道路標識--高速道路編("Instruction interministerielle sur la Signalisation Routiere(Livre 2.8 Mars 1971)--Signalisation des Autoroutes") , 1972 .

[12]  Patricia Delhomme,et al.  What factors can predict why drivers go through yellow traffic lights ? An approach based on an extended Theory of Planned Behavior , 2012 .

[13]  David Shinar,et al.  Aggressive driving: the contribution of the drivers and the situation , 1998 .

[14]  P. Hancock,et al.  How cognitive load affects duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[15]  J. Averill Personal control over aversive stimuli and its relationship to stress. , 1973 .

[16]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment , 2002 .

[17]  Ola Svenson,et al.  Judgments of mean speed and predictions of route choice , 2011 .

[18]  Stuart Marcovitch,et al.  Sequence learning in infancy: the independent contributions of conditional probability and pair frequency information. , 2009, Developmental science.

[19]  S. Epstein,et al.  Conflict between intuitive and rational processing: when people behave against their better judgment. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  P. Delhomme,et al.  Accessibilité des croyances associées aux deux options d'une alternative comportementale , 2006 .

[21]  Dominik Papinski,et al.  Exploring the route choice decision-making process: A comparison of planned and observed routes obtained using person-based GPS , 2009 .

[22]  Ola Svenson,et al.  Driving speed changes and subjective estimates of time savings, accident risks and braking , 2009 .

[23]  Ola Svenson,et al.  Decisions among time saving options: when intuition is strong and wrong. , 2008, Acta psychologica.

[24]  Patricia Delhomme,et al.  Speed behaviour as a choice between observing and exceeding the speed limit , 2005 .

[25]  W. Estes The cognitive side of probability learning. , 1976 .

[26]  S. Sutherland Irrationality: The Enemy Within , 1992 .

[27]  R. S. Lynch,et al.  Development of a Driving Anger Scale , 1994, Psychological reports.

[28]  S. Epstein Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. , 1994, The American psychologist.

[29]  S Epstein,et al.  Cognitive-experiential self-theory and subjective probability: further evidence for two conceptual systems. , 1992, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[30]  Talib Rothengatter,et al.  Risk and the absence of pleasure: a motivational approach to modelling road user behaviour , 1988 .

[31]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Dual-process theories in social psychology , 1999 .

[32]  Alan Agresti,et al.  Categorical Data Analysis , 1991, International Encyclopedia of Statistical Science.

[33]  F Letirand,et al.  Augmenter la valeur predictive de la theorie du comportement planifie par l'evaluation de plusieurs options comportementales : Une application au comportement de vitesse sur la route , 2003 .

[34]  H. Summala HIERARCHICAL MODEL OF BEHAVIOURAL ADAPTATION AND TRAFFIC ACCIDENTS , 1997 .

[35]  Bernard Deceuninck Ministère de l'Ecologie, de l'Energie, du Développement Durable et de la Mer , 2010 .