The (in)dependence of articulation and lexical planning during isolated word production
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Jennifer E. Arnold,et al. Tic Tac TOE: Effects of predictability and importance on acoustic prominence in language production , 2008, Cognition.
[2] Miguel A. Perez. Age of acquisition persists as the main factor in picture naming when cumulative word frequency and frequency trajectory are controlled , 2007, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[3] Gregory R. Guy. From and function in linguistic variation , 1996 .
[4] T. Jaeger,et al. Production preferences cannot be understood without reference to communication , 2013, Front. Psychol..
[5] A. Meyer. The time course of phonological encoding in language production: Phonological encoding inside a syllable , 1991 .
[6] E. Yund,et al. Measuring consonant identification in nonsense syllables, words, and sentences. , 2010, Journal of rehabilitation research and development.
[7] Mariapaola D'Imperio,et al. Lexical and contextual predictability: Confluent effects on the production of vowels , 2010 .
[8] Julie E. Boland,et al. Priming in pronunciation: Beyond pattern recognition and onset latency , 1989 .
[9] M. Aylett,et al. Language redundancy predicts syllabic duration and the spectral characteristics of vocalic syllable nuclei. , 2006, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[10] Alice Turk,et al. The Smooth Signal Redundancy Hypothesis: A Functional Explanation for Relationships between Redundancy, Prosodic Prominence, and Duration in Spontaneous Speech , 2004, Language and speech.
[11] Victor S Ferreira,et al. Phonological Influences on Lexical (Mis)Selection , 2003, Psychological science.
[12] A. Wedel. Exemplar models, evolution and language change , 2006 .
[13] P. Luce,et al. Probabilistic Phonotactics and Neighborhood Activation in Spoken Word Recognition , 1999 .
[14] Rebecca Scarborough,et al. Lexical similarity and speech production: Neighborhoods for nonwords , 2012 .
[15] Steven T Piantadosi,et al. Word lengths are optimized for efficient communication , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[16] S. Brennan,et al. Attenuating Information in Spoken Communication: For the Speaker, or for the Addressee?. , 2010 .
[17] E. Bard,et al. Controlling the Intelligibility of Referring Expressions in Dialogue , 2000 .
[18] G. Dell,et al. The sequential cuing effect in speech production , 1994, Cognition.
[19] Ann R Bradlow,et al. Variability in Word Duration as a Function of Probability, Speech Style, and Prosody , 2009, Language and speech.
[20] S. G. Guion,et al. Clear speech production of Korean stops: changing phonetic targets and enhancement strategies. , 2008, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[21] Matthew Goldrick,et al. The effects of lexical neighbors on stop consonant articulation. , 2013, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[22] K. Bock. An Effect of the Accessibility of Word Forms on Sentence Structures , 1987 .
[23] Dmitrii Manin,et al. Experiments on predictability of word in context and information rate in natural language , 2006, ArXiv.
[24] Kyle Gorman,et al. Prosodylab-aligner: A tool for forced alignment of laboratory speech , 2011 .
[25] Holly P. Branigan,et al. Contributions of animacy to grammatical function assignment and word order during production , 2008 .
[26] James S. Magnuson,et al. The Dynamics of Lexical Competition During Spoken Word Recognition , 2007, Cogn. Sci..
[27] Christo Kirov,et al. The Specificity of Online Variation in Speech Production , 2012, CogSci.
[28] Amanda Stent,et al. Adapting speaking after evidence of misrecognition: Local and global hyperarticulation , 2008, Speech Commun..
[29] Jessamyn Schertz,et al. Exaggeration of featural contrasts in clarifications of misheard speech in English , 2013, J. Phonetics.
[30] Esteban Buz,et al. Dynamic hyperarticulation of coda voicing contrasts. , 2016, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[31] Jean E. Fox Tree,et al. Pronouncing “the” as “thee” to signal problems in speaking , 1997, Cognition.
[32] Keith Johnson,et al. Resonance in an exemplar-based lexicon: The emergence of social identity and phonology , 2006, J. Phonetics.
[33] Christopher T. Kello,et al. Parallel processing and initial phoneme criterion in naming words : Evidence from frequency effects on onset and rime duration , 1999 .
[34] Thomas Wasow. End-Weight from the Speaker's Perspective , 1997 .
[35] Neal P. Fox,et al. Phonological Neighborhood Competition Affects Spoken Word Production Irrespective of Sentential Context. , 2015, Journal of memory and language.
[36] Duane G. Watson,et al. Repetition is easy: Why repeated referents have reduced prominence , 2010, Memory & cognition.
[37] Jake Kurczek,et al. The ageing neighbourhood: phonological density in naming , 2014, Language and cognitive processes.
[38] John J. Ohala,et al. Discussion of Björn Lindblom's ‘Phonetic Invariance and the Adaptive Nature of Speech’ , 1989 .
[39] T. Florian Jaeger,et al. Redundancy and reduction: Speakers manage syntactic information density , 2010, Cognitive Psychology.
[40] B. Lindblom,et al. Role of articulation in speech perception: clues from production. , 1996, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[41] Michael K. Tanenhaus,et al. Contextual confusability leads to targeted hyperarticulation , 2014, CogSci.
[42] Matthew Goldrick,et al. Grammatical constraints on phonological encoding in speech production , 2014, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[43] Antje S. Meyer,et al. Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production : Picture word interference studies , 1990 .
[44] Michael S Vitevitch,et al. The facilitative influence of phonological similarity and neighborhood frequency in speech production in younger and older adults , 2003, Memory & cognition.
[45] P. O'Seaghdha,et al. Phonological competition and cooperation in form-related priming: sequential and nonsequential processes in word production. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[46] Maryellen C. MacDonald,et al. How language production shapes language form and comprehension , 2012, Front. Psychol..
[47] M. Vitevitch,et al. Sublexical and lexical representations in speech production: effects of phonotactic probability and onset density. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[48] D. Plaut,et al. The task dependence of staged versus cascaded processing: an empirical and computational study of Stroop interference in speech production. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[49] Jennifer E. Arnold,et al. A processing-centered look at the contribution of givenness to durational reduction , 2012 .
[50] Sarah C. Creel,et al. Heeding the voice of experience: The role of talker variation in lexical access , 2008, Cognition.
[51] Jennifer E. Arnold,et al. Articulatory and lexical repetition effects on durational reduction: speaker experience vs. common ground , 2015 .
[52] Rebecca Scarborough,et al. Clarity in communication: "clear" speech authenticity and lexical neighborhood density effects in speech production and perception. , 2013, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[53] R. Wright. Phonetic Interpretation Papers in Laboratory Phonology VI: Factors of lexical competition in vowel articulation , 2004 .
[54] R. Baayen,et al. Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items , 2008 .
[55] Michael S Vitevitch,et al. The influence of phonological similarity neighborhoods on speech production. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[56] A. House. On Vowel Duration in English , 1961 .
[57] T. Florian Jaeger,et al. Incremental Phonological Encoding during Unscripted Sentence Production , 2012, Front. Psychology.
[58] A. House,et al. Characterization and modeling of speech-segment durations , 1986, ICASSP '86. IEEE International Conference on Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Processing.
[59] D. Pisoni,et al. Recognizing Spoken Words: The Neighborhood Activation Model , 1998, Ear and hearing.
[60] T Florian Jaeger,et al. Phonological overlap affects lexical selection during sentence production. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[61] Matthew Goldrick,et al. Mechanisms of interaction in speech production , 2009, Language and cognitive processes.
[62] Victor S Ferreira,et al. Given-New Ordering Effects on the Production of Scrambled Sentences in Japanese , 2003, Journal of psycholinguistic research.
[63] M. Tanenhaus,et al. Approaches to studying world-situated language use : bridging the language-as-product and language-as-action traditions , 2005 .
[64] Elizabeth Shriberg. DISFLUENCIES IN SWITCHBOARD , 1996 .
[65] K. Drager. Speaker Age and Vowel Perception , 2011, Language and speech.
[66] R. Newman,et al. The role of selected lexical factors on confrontation naming accuracy, speed, and fluency in adults who do and do not stutter. , 2007, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[67] Janet B. Pierrehumbert,et al. Word-specific phonetics , 2001 .
[68] Yuan Zhao,et al. The effect of lexical frequency and Lombard reflex on tone hyperarticulation , 2009, J. Phonetics.
[69] Kara D. Federmeier,et al. Timed picture naming in seven languages , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[70] A. Lahiri,et al. Prosodic Units in Speech Production , 1997 .
[71] Sarah Brown-Schmidt,et al. The rapid use of gender information: evidence of the time course of pronoun resolution from eyetracking , 2000, Cognition.
[72] G. Dell,et al. Effect of Ambiguity and Lexical Availability on Syntactic and Lexical Production , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.
[73] Albert Costa,et al. Neighbourhood density and frequency effects in speech production: A case for interactivity , 2008 .
[74] Christo Kirov,et al. Bayesian Speech Production: Evidence from Latency and Hyperarticulation , 2013, CogSci.
[75] Björn Lindblom,et al. Explaining Phonetic Variation: A Sketch of the H&H Theory , 1990 .
[76] T. Florian Jaeger,et al. Signal Reduction and Linguistic Encoding , 2017 .
[77] H. H. Clark,et al. Using uh and um in spontaneous speaking , 2002, Cognition.
[78] Susanne Gahl,et al. Lexical competition in vowel articulation revisited: Vowel dispersion in the Easy/Hard database , 2015, J. Phonetics.
[79] G S Dell,et al. A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. , 1986, Psychological review.
[80] Duane G. Watson,et al. The Effect of Phonological Encoding on Word Duration: Selection Takes Time , 2015 .
[81] David L Woods,et al. Consonant identification in consonant-vowel-consonant syllables in speech-spectrum noise. , 2010, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[82] B. Rapp,et al. Discreteness and interactivity in spoken word production. , 2000, Psychological review.
[83] William Labov,et al. Towards a social science of language: papers in honour of William Labov , 1999 .
[84] B. Munson,et al. The effect of phonological neighborhood density on vowel articulation. , 2004, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[85] H. Traunmüller. Analytical expressions for the tonotopic sensory scale , 1990 .
[86] Duane G. Watson,et al. Why are repeated words produced with reduced durations? Evidence from inner speech and homophone production. , 2015, Journal of memory and language.
[87] Jason M. Brenier,et al. Predictability Effects on Durations of Content and Function Words in Conversational English , 2009 .
[88] M. Damian. Articulatory duration in single-word speech production. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[89] Julia F Strand,et al. Phi-square Lexical Competition Database (Phi-Lex): An online tool for quantifying auditory and visual lexical competition , 2014, Behavior research methods.
[90] Keith Johnson,et al. Why reduce? Phonological neighborhood density and phonetic reduction in spontaneous speech , 2012 .
[91] Melissa K. Stamer,et al. The curious case of competition in Spanish speech production , 2006, Language and cognitive processes.
[92] Janet B. Pierrehumbert,et al. Exemplar dynamics: Word frequency, lenition and contrast , 2000 .
[93] Melissa Baese-Berk,et al. Phonological Neighborhood Effects in Spoken Word Production: An fMRI Study , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[94] Willem J. M. Levelt,et al. A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[95] Jennifer E. Arnold,et al. Reference production: Production-internal and addressee-oriented processes , 2008 .
[96] Melissa K. Stamer,et al. The influence of neighborhood density (and neighborhood frequency) in Spanish speech production: A follow-up report , 2009 .
[97] Marc Brysbaert,et al. Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English , 2009, Behavior research methods.
[98] B. Munson. Lexical Access , Lexical Representation , and Vowel Production , 2006 .
[99] P. Luce,et al. When Words Compete: Levels of Processing in Perception of Spoken Words , 1998 .
[100] Bradford Z. Mahon,et al. Lexical selection is not by competition: a reinterpretation of semantic interference and facilitation effects in the picture-word interference paradigm. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[101] Christopher T. Kello,et al. Control over the time course of cognition in the tempo-naming task. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[102] Jennifer E. Arnold,et al. Synthesising meaning and processing approaches to prosody: performance matters , 2015, Language, cognition and neuroscience.
[103] D. Mirman,et al. Competition and cooperation among similar representations: toward a unified account of facilitative and inhibitory effects of lexical neighbors. , 2012, Psychological review.
[104] A. Meyer. The time course of phonological encoding in language production: The encoding of successive syllables of a word ☆ , 1990 .
[105] Clara D. Martin,et al. Reconciling Phonological Neighborhood Effects in Speech Production through Single Trial Analysis Reconciling Phonological Neighborhood Effects in Speech Production through Single Trial Analysis , 2022 .
[106] Rebecca Scarborough,et al. Neighborhood-conditioned patterns in phonetic detail: Relating coarticulation and hyperarticulation , 2013, J. Phonetics.
[107] Jennifer E. Arnold,et al. Audience design affects acoustic reduction via production facilitation , 2012, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[108] J. Hay. Lexical frequency in morphology: Is everything relative? , 2001 .