The Neuroergonomics of Vigilance

Objective: The aim of this study was to examine the effects of uncertainty about where in the field of view critical signals for detection appear during a vigilance task (spatial uncertainty) on cerebral blood flow velocity (CBFV) and oculomotor fatigue. Background: Neuroergonomics is a dimension of human factors founded by Raja Parasuraman that studies brain functions underlying performance at work. Neuroergonomic studies have shown that observers in vigilance tasks lose information-processing resources over time and experience oculomotor fatigue as indexed by a temporal decline in CBFV and elevation in eye closure as reflected in the PERCLOS metric. Because spatial uncertainty increases an observer’s need for visual scanning relative to a spatial certainty condition, it was anticipated that spatial uncertainty would result in a greater temporal decline in CBFV and increased eye closure in a vigilance session. Method: Observers performed a simulated unmanned aerial vehicle (UAV) control task wherein collision flight paths were the events to be detected. UAV images could appear at random in any one of five locations on the controller’s display (spatial uncertainty) or only in a fixed location (spatial certainty). Results: Signal detection was poorer in the spatial-uncertain relative to the certain condition, and predictions regarding CBFV and eye closure were confirmed. Conclusion: Vigilance tasks involving spatial uncertainty are more neurophysiologically taxing than those in which spatial uncertainty is not a factor. Application: The neuroergonomic approach helps in understanding the effects of psychophysical factors in vigilance and to signify when performance aiding is needed.

[1]  Scott E. Maxwell,et al.  Designing Experiments and Analyzing Data: A Model Comparison Perspective , 1990 .

[2]  G. Matthews Human Performance: Cognition, Stress and Individual Differences , 2000 .

[3]  Marcus E. Raichle,et al.  Circulatory and Metabolic Correlates of Brain Function in Normal Humans , 2011 .

[4]  R. Schandry,et al.  Functional transcranial Doppler sonography as a tool in psychophysiological research. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[5]  Lauren Reinerman-Jones,et al.  Detection tasks in nuclear power plant operation: Vigilance decrement and physiological workload monitoring , 2016 .

[6]  D. Schroeder,et al.  Blink Rate: A Possible Measure of Fatigue , 1994, Human factors.

[7]  Hung T. Nguyen,et al.  Comparing features extractors in EEG-based cognitive fatigue detection of demanding computer tasks , 2015, 2015 37th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC).

[8]  Edward M. Hitchcock,et al.  Automation cueing modulates cerebral blood flow and vigilance in a simulated air traffic control task , 2003 .

[9]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Neuroergonomics , 2011 .

[10]  Lynn C. Robertson,et al.  A Review of Hemispheric Asymmetry: What's Right and What's Left , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  M. Raichle Behind the scenes of functional brain imaging: a historical and physiological perspective. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[12]  G L Shulman,et al.  Blood flow and oxygen delivery to human brain during functional activity: Theoretical modeling and experimental data , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[13]  Joel S. Warm,et al.  Event-related cerebral hemodynamics reveal target-specific resource allocation for both “go” and “no-go” response-based vigilance tasks , 2013, Brain and Cognition.

[14]  E. Stein,et al.  Multiple Neuronal Networks Mediate Sustained Attention , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  D. Dinges,et al.  EVALUATION OF TECHNIQUES FOR OCULAR MEASUREMENT AS AN INDEX OF FATIGUE AND THE BASIS FOR ALERTNESS MANAGEMENT , 1998 .

[16]  Tyler H. Shaw,et al.  Effects of regular or irregular event schedules on cerebral hemovelocity during a sustained attention task , 2012, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[17]  Joel S. Warm,et al.  Effects of Stereoscopic Depth on Vigilance Performance and Cerebral Hemodynamics , 2015, Hum. Factors.

[18]  J. Jonides Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind's eye's movement , 1981 .

[19]  Jason S. McCarley,et al.  Eye Movements as a Window on Perception and Cognition , 2006, Neuroergonomics.

[20]  R. A. Bailey Designing Experiments and Analyzing Data: a Model Comparison Perspective, 2nd edn , 2005 .

[21]  Jason S. McCarley,et al.  Oculomotor behaviour as a reflection of attention and memory processes: Neural mechanisms and applications to human factors , 2003 .

[22]  W. Helton,et al.  Global interference and spatial uncertainty in the Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART) , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.

[23]  G. Wilson,et al.  Measurement of Operator Workload with the Neuropsychological Workload Test Battery , 1988 .

[24]  Nam-Ho Kim,et al.  Distribution Statement A: Approved for Public Release , 2016 .

[25]  Joost C. F. de Winter,et al.  Review of eye-related measures of drivers’ mental workload , 2015 .

[26]  R. Parasuraman,et al.  A Taxonomic Analysis of Vigilance Performance , 1977 .

[27]  Joel S. Warm,et al.  Cerebral Hemodynamics and Vigilance Performance , 2009 .

[28]  Thomas Loveday,et al.  Cue-Based Processing Capacity, Cognitive Load and the Completion of Simulated Short-Duration Vigilance Tasks in Power Transmission Control , 2014 .

[29]  M. Loeb,et al.  The Psychology of Vigilance , 1982 .

[30]  Joel S. Warm,et al.  Static and Dynamic Discriminations in Vigilance: Effects on Cerebral Hemodynamics and Workload , 2010 .

[31]  R Parasuraman,et al.  Aging and cognitive vigilance: effects of spatial uncertainty and event rate. , 1995, Experimental aging research.

[32]  H. Bastian Sensation and Perception.—I , 1869, Nature.

[33]  Joel S. Warm,et al.  Vigilance, Workload, and Stress , 2018 .

[34]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Attention and Effort , 1973 .

[35]  D. Navon Resources—a theoretical soup stone? , 1984 .

[36]  Joel S. Warm,et al.  Vigilance Requires Hard Mental Work and Is Stressful , 2008, Hum. Factors.

[37]  D. Washburn,et al.  A transcranial Doppler sonography study of shoot/don’t-shoot responding , 2009, Behavior research methods.

[38]  Joel S. Warm,et al.  Selection for vigilance assignments: a review and proposed new direction , 2011 .

[39]  Petra Holtzmann Sustained Attention In Human Performance , 2016 .

[40]  J. A. Adams,et al.  SPATIAL AND TEMPORAL UNCERTAINTY AS DETERMINANTS OF VIGILANCE BEHAVIOR. , 1964, Journal of experimental psychology.

[41]  Tyler H. Shaw,et al.  Effects of sensory modality on cerebral blood flow velocity during vigilance , 2009, Neuroscience Letters.

[42]  R J Fairbanks,et al.  RESEARCH ON VEHICLE-BASED DRIVER STATUS/PERFORMANCE MONITORING; DEVELOPMENT, VALIDATION, AND REFINEMENT OF ALGORITHMS FOR DETECTION OF DRIVER DROWSINESS. FINAL REPORT , 1994 .

[43]  F. Jacob Seagull,et al.  Methods and applications of eye tracking , 2015 .

[44]  M Allen Models of hemispheric specialization. , 1983, Psychological bulletin.

[45]  Kenneth Holmqvist,et al.  Eye tracking: a comprehensive guide to methods and measures , 2011 .

[46]  L. Heytens,et al.  Transcranial Doppler sonography. , 1989, Acta neurologica Belgica.

[47]  James L. Szalma,et al.  Perception and Attention , 2015 .

[48]  C. H. Baker Attention to visual displays during a vigilance task. I. Biasing attention. , 1958, British journal of psychology.

[49]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Applied perception and neuroergonomics , 2015 .

[50]  Joel S. Warm,et al.  The Cambridge Handbook of Applied Perception Research: Vigilance: A Perceptual Challenge , 2015 .

[51]  I. Robertson,et al.  The absent mind: further investigations of sustained attention to response , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[52]  Richard J. Hanowski,et al.  The impact of local/short haul operations on driver fatigue , 2000 .

[53]  W. N. Dember,et al.  Effects of Multiple-Signal Discrimination on Vigilance Performance and Perceived Workload , 1995 .

[54]  Joel S. Warm,et al.  Feature Presence/Absence Modifies the Event Rate Effect and Cerebral Hemovelocity in Vigilance Performance , 2004 .

[55]  G. A. Miller,et al.  Some effects of unequal spatial distribution on the detectability of radar targets. , 1957, Journal of experimental psychology.

[56]  P. Hancock,et al.  The Cambridge handbook of applied perception research , 2015 .

[57]  E. Crelin Atlas of Human Anatomy , 1965, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.

[58]  I. Robertson,et al.  `Oops!': Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[59]  Uta Boehm,et al.  Attention Theory And Practice , 2016 .

[60]  Guy Vingerhoets,et al.  Transcranial Doppler Ultrasonography Monitoring of Cerebral Hemodynamics During Performance of Cognitive Tasks: A Review , 2000, Neuropsychology Review.

[61]  R. Gonzalez Applied Multivariate Statistics for the Social Sciences , 2003 .

[62]  D. Washburn,et al.  Task engagement, cerebral blood flow velocity, and diagnostic monitoring for sustained attention. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[63]  D. Besner,et al.  A Resource-Control Account of Sustained Attention , 2015, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[64]  William S. Helton,et al.  Sustained attention to local and global target features is different: Performance and tympanic membrane temperature , 2009, Brain and Cognition.

[65]  Michel Besserve,et al.  Multimodal information improves the rapid detection of mental fatigue , 2013, Biomed. Signal Process. Control..

[66]  Roger E. Kirk,et al.  Experimental design: Procedures for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). , 1995 .

[67]  Staniŝa Miloŝević,et al.  Effect of time and space uncertainty on a vigilance task , 1974 .

[68]  Joel S. Warm,et al.  The Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART) Does Not Promote Mindlessness During Vigilance Performance , 2014, Hum. Factors.

[69]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Neuroergonomics: Research and practice , 2003 .

[70]  Joel S. Warm,et al.  The Psychophysics of Vigilance , 1980 .

[71]  R A Bell,et al.  Watchkeeping Performance as a Function of Certain Properties of the Viewing Situation , 1974, Human factors.

[72]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Introduction to Neuroergonomics , 2009, Neuroergonomics.

[73]  Judi E. See,et al.  Brain systems of vigilance. , 1998 .

[74]  M. M. Lorist,et al.  Considerations of the influence of mental fatigue on controlled and automatic cognitive processing and related neuromodulatory effects , 2011 .

[75]  William S. Helton,et al.  Effects of breaks and goal switches on the vigilance decrement , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.

[76]  C. Wickens Engineering psychology and human performance, 2nd ed. , 1992 .

[77]  Joel S. Warm,et al.  Cerebral Hemodynamics and Vigilance , 2006, Neuroergonomics.

[78]  J. G. Hollands,et al.  Engineering Psychology and Human Performance , 1984 .

[79]  Joel S. Warm,et al.  Perceived Time Progression and Vigilance: Implications for Workload, Stress, and Cerebral Hemodynamics , 2013 .

[80]  Saman K. Halgamuge,et al.  Driver Fatigue Detection by Fusing Multiple Cues , 2007, ISNN.

[81]  Joel S. Warm,et al.  Cerebral Hemodynamic Indices of Operator Fatigue in Vigilance , 2017 .

[82]  Earl A. Alluisi,et al.  Effects of stimulus-response uncertainty on watchkeeping performance and choice reactions , 1967 .

[83]  C. H. Baker Attention to visual displays during a vigilance task. II. Maintaining the level of vigilance. , 1959, British journal of psychology.

[84]  R. Aaslid Transcranial Doppler Examination Techniques , 1986 .