Being and Time: Judged Presence and Duration as a Function of Media Form

We briefly describe a novel immersive environmentthe interactive tentand an artistic production within it, the Illusion of Being. In this production, immersants experience a vivid cycle of the elements in a way that depends on their bodily movements. This elemental story has four versions in all, with each created to have differential effects on sense of presence and of subjective duration, according to a theoretical model. The model proposes three orthogonal dimensions of experience: focus, the level of abstraction;locus, real versus virtual; andsensus, the overall level of attention of the observer. An experiment, conducted to assess the effects of the different versions, is reported. The results confirmed the model's prediction that rated presence is relatively high when experienced media is of a form that elicits predominantly concrete (perceptual) processing, and relatively low when the emphasis is on more-abstract (conceptual) processing. But the concrete-abstract dimension had no direct effect on judged duration, contrary to our predictions. However, some evidence suggests that judged presence and estimated duration were positively correlated for media categorized as virtual, but not for content captured from the real world.

[1]  Hanna Holmgren,et al.  Presence and Duration - The Relationship between the Sense of presence and Subjective Duration , 2000 .

[2]  Woodrow Barfield,et al.  A Conceptual Model of the Sense of Presence in Virtual Environments , 1999, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[3]  Mel Slater,et al.  Depth of Presence in Virtual Environments , 1994, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[4]  J. Falk,et al.  Development of Visual Preference for Natural Environments , 1982 .

[5]  John M. Flach,et al.  Critical Ratios as Behavioural Indices of Presence , 1999 .

[6]  Susumu Tachi,et al.  Water dome-an augmented environment , 2000, 2000 IEEE Conference on Information Visualization. An International Conference on Computer Visualization and Graphics.

[7]  Alan D. Baddeley,et al.  Verbal Reasoning and Working Memory , 1976 .

[8]  Peter Paolucci,et al.  Designing Information Spaces: The Social Navigation Approach , 2003, J. Educ. Technol. Soc..

[9]  Sandra Blakeslee,et al.  Phantoms in the Brain , 1998 .

[10]  Wijnand A. IJsselsteijn,et al.  Presence: concept, determinants, and measurement , 2000, Electronic Imaging.

[11]  A. Damasio The feeling of what happens , 2001 .

[12]  G. Riva,et al.  Virtual Environments in Clinical Psychology and Neuroscience: Methods and Techniques in Advanced Patient-Therapist Interaction , 1998 .

[13]  Naoko Tosa Unconscious flow , 1999, SIGGRAPH '99.

[14]  B. Baars A cognitive theory of consciousness , 1988 .

[15]  Andreas Lund,et al.  Experiential Design of Shared Information Spaces , 2003, Designing Information Spaces.

[16]  Wa Wijnand IJsselsteijn,et al.  Duration Estimation and Presence , 2001 .

[17]  Gregory A. Smith,et al.  Danger, Mystery, and Environmental Preference , 1988 .

[18]  Michael G. Flaherty,et al.  A Watched Pot: How We Experience Time , 1999 .

[19]  Eva Lindh Waterworth,et al.  Focus, Locus, and Sensus: The Three Dimensions of Virtual Experience , 2001, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[20]  Isabel Góis,et al.  Understanding Consciousness , 2001 .

[21]  N. Humphrey A History of the Mind , 1992 .

[22]  Holger Regenbrecht,et al.  The Experience of Presence: Factor Analytic Insights , 2001, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[23]  Mel Slater,et al.  A Virtual Presence Counter , 2000, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[24]  Abi Berger,et al.  Phantoms in the brain , 1999, BMJ.

[25]  J A Waterworth,et al.  Memory Mechanisms and the Psychophysical Scaling of Duration , 1985, Perception.