Language Abstraction: Consolidation of Language Structure During Sleep

Language Abstraction: Consolidation of Language Structure During Sleep Michelle C. St. Clair (michelle.stclair@manchester.ac.uk) School of Psychological Sciences, University of Manchester Manchester, M13 9PL, UK Padraic Monaghan (p.monaghan@lancaster.ac.uk) Department of Psychology, University of Lancaster Lancaster, LA1 4YF, UK However, before positing specific language-learning mechanisms or innately-specified linguistic structure, it is important to do full justice to the potential richness of general purpose learning mechanisms for the acquisition of language structure. There are many candidates for studies showing the great potential of linguistic input (Monaghan, Christiansen, & Chater, 2007) and the versatility of general learning mechanisms that can generate structure from the language itself (e.g., Reali, Christiansen, & Monaghan, 2003). In the latter study, a simple recurrent network was trained to learn the patterns of associations between distributional and phonological cues for categorising grammatical categories in speech. So, a fundamental question for language acquisition research is what are the range of general purpose learning mechanisms available to the language learner, and how do they apply to language input? There is accumulating evidence that there are critical qualitative distinctions in the effects of learning new material when assessed before compared to after sleep, and specifically that sleep may facilitate abstraction and generalisation in a range of tasks (e.g., Fischer, Drosopoulos, Tsen, & Born, 2006; Gomez, Bootzin, & Nadel, 2006; Wagner, Gais, Haider, Verleger, & Born, 2004). Although only a few sleep studies have used linguistic tasks, the underlying structures and features of the stimuli used in many of these studies are strikingly similar to the critical features of natural and artificial languages. The current study advances the established field of language learning studies by making use of innovative recent methods for investigating the cognitive influences of sleep on complex learning. We contend that sleep elicits critical general purpose learning that is important for the process of language acquisition. Abstract Recent research on how sleep influences cognitive processes has pointed to the potential role of sleep in the consolidation and generalisation of information across a range of tasks, including some language learning tasks (i.e., Gomez, Bootzin, & Nadel, 2006; Wagner, Gais, Haider, Verleger, & Born, 2004; Walker, 2005). The current study investigated the role of sleep in grammatical category generalisation by testing participants for categorical knowledge 12 hours after initial training. Participants were tested on their categorisation of the training words and also on their ability to generalize to novel, but consistent category words. The sleep group received training at 9pm and the wake group at 9am. Only the sleep group was able to abstract the general category characteristics to the novel category words which implies a domain general abstraction mechanism may be influential in language acquisition. Introduction The child’s language environment provides an immensely rich source of information that can contribute to the development of structure at the word level (Saffran, Aslin, & Newport, 1996) and to the development of grammatical categories (Redington, Chater, & Finch, 1998) and syntactic constraints (Bod, 2007). From a sea of sounds (Saffran, 2001), the child must learn where words begin and end in continuous speech, and the child must also learn the constraints on the relations between words, as described by grammatical structure. One of the central issues in the cognitive science of language over the last 50 years is to determine the extent to which the language environment triggers innately-specified, domain-specific processing (Chomsky, 2006; Pinker, 1991), and the extent to which sufficient information is present within the signal to provide structure via general-purpose learning mechanisms (Conway & Christiansen, 2005; Saffran, 2002). Chomsky (1965, 1980) proposed that the linguistic input that a child is exposed to provides insufficient information to adequately constrain the actual grammar of the language that the child develops (Gold, 1967). For instance, children are unlikely to be exposed to instances of auxiliary-fronting (e.g., converting “the dog that is sleeping is grey” to a question format: “is the dog that is sleeping grey” by moving the correct auxiliary verb). The debate over whether general-purpose learning mechanisms can provide sufficient, indirect constraints to distinguish between grammatical versus ungrammatical constructions is a matter of current contention (see, e.g., Messer et al., 1995; Reali & Christiansen, 2005). Sleep Consolidation and Qualitative Changes in Learning Sleep has been found to play a pivotal role in memory consolidation, both in terms of declarative and procedural memory, as well as being implicated in brain plasticity for storage (see Walker, 2005 for a review). Increases in perceptual skills and in explicit knowledge about complex tasks has also been associated with sleep (Fischer, Drosopoulos, Tsen, & Born, 2006; Wagner, Gais, Haider, Verleger, & Born, 2004). Wagner et al. (2004) conducted a study to test learning of abstract structure in strings of digits, where the last three digits were the second, third and fourth digits in reverse order (e.g. 1914419). After an eight hour

[1]  Rens Bod,et al.  The Data-Oriented Parsing Approach: Theory and Application , 2008, Computational Intelligence: A Compendium.

[2]  Matthew P Walker,et al.  A refined model of sleep and the time course of memory formation. , 2005, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[3]  M. Gaskell,et al.  Sleep-Associated Changes in the Mental Representation of Spoken Words , 2007, Psychological science.

[4]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  The phonological-distributional coherence hypothesis: Cross-linguistic evidence in language acquisition , 2007, Cognitive Psychology.

[5]  Rebecca L. Gómez,et al.  Naps Promote Abstraction in Language-Learning Infants , 2006, Psychological science.

[6]  Adrian Angold,et al.  Development of a Short Questionnaire for Use in Epidemiological Studies of Depression in Children and Adolescents: Factor Composition and Structure Across Development. , 1995 .

[7]  E. Mark Gold,et al.  Language Identification in the Limit , 1967, Inf. Control..

[8]  D. J. Marshall,et al.  Measuring the Social Competence of Preschool Children With Specific Language Impairment , 2006 .

[9]  H. Nusbaum,et al.  Consolidation during sleep of perceptual learning of spoken language , 2003, Nature.

[10]  Patricia J. Brooks,et al.  Exploring language acquisition in children with a miniature artificial language: Effects of item and pattern frequency, arbitrary subclasses, and correction☆ , 1990 .

[11]  Patricia J. Brooks,et al.  Acquisition of Gender-like Noun Subclasses in an Artificial Language: The Contribution of Phonological Markers to Learning , 1993 .

[12]  J. Born,et al.  Learning-Dependent Increases in Sleep Spindle Density , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  Hajime Murai,et al.  Categorizing Grammar: Differential Effects of Preceding and Succeeding Contextual Cues , 2005 .

[14]  Jenny R Saffran,et al.  Words in a sea of sounds: the output of infant statistical learning , 2001, Cognition.

[15]  Nick Chater,et al.  Distributional Information: A Powerful Cue for Acquiring Syntactic Categories , 1998, Cogn. Sci..

[16]  M. Goldsmith,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996 .

[17]  Virginia Valian,et al.  Anchor points in language learning: The role of marker frequency ☆ , 1988 .

[18]  C. Smith,et al.  Sleep spindles and learning potential. , 2007, Behavioral neuroscience.

[19]  J. Born,et al.  Early sleep triggers memory for early visual discrimination skills , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  J. Born,et al.  Sleep inspires insight , 2004, Nature.

[21]  S Pinker,et al.  Rules of language. , 1991, Science.

[22]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Modality-constrained statistical learning of tactile, visual, and auditory sequences. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[23]  M. H. Kelly,et al.  Phonological information for grammatical category assignments , 1991 .

[24]  St. Clair,et al.  Language structure and language acquisition: grammatical categorization using phonological and distributional information , 2007 .

[25]  J. Saffran Constraints on Statistical Language Learning , 2002 .

[26]  M. H. Kelly,et al.  Children’s use of phonology to infer grammatical class in vocabulary learning , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[27]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Uncovering the Richness of the Stimulus: Structure Dependence and Indirect Statistical Evidence , 2005, Cogn. Sci..

[28]  Jan Born,et al.  Implicit Learning–Explicit Knowing: A Role for Sleep in Memory System Interaction , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[29]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  The differential role of phonological and distributional cues in grammatical categorisation , 2005, Cognition.

[30]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  वाक्यविन्यास का सैद्धान्तिक पक्ष = Aspects of the theory of syntax , 1965 .

[31]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Phonological and Distributional Cues in Syntax Acquisition: Scaling up the Connectionist Approach to Multiple-Cue Integration - eScholarship , 2003 .