Use-Dependent Plasticity Explains Aftereffects in Visually Guided Locomotor Learning of a Novel Step Length Asymmetry.

Studies of upper extremity reaching show that use-dependent plasticity, or learning from repetition, plays an important role in shaping motor behaviors. Yet, the impact of repetition on locomotor learning is unclear, despite the fact that gait is developed and practiced over millions of repetitions. To test if repetition alone can induce storage of a novel walking pattern, we instructed two groups of young healthy subjects to learn an asymmetric walking pattern through two distinct learning paradigms. The first group learned a new pattern through an established visual distortion paradigm, which provided both sensory prediction error and repetition of movement patterns to induce walking aftereffects, and the second received veridical feedback with a target change, which provided only repetition (use-dependent plasticity) to induce aftereffects. When feedback was removed, both groups demonstrated aftereffects in the primary outcome, step asymmetry index. Surprisingly, despite the different task demands, both groups produced similar aftereffect magnitudes, which also had similar rates of decay, suggesting that the addition of sensory prediction errors did not improve storage of learning beyond that induced by the use-dependent process alone. To further characterize the use-dependent process, we conducted a second experiment to quantify aftereffect size in a third group that practiced double the asymmetry magnitude. This new group showed a proportionately greater magnitude of the use-dependent aftereffect. Together, these findings show that the primary driver of storage of a new step length asymmetry during visually-guided locomotor learning is repetition, not sensory prediction error, and this effect scales with the learning magnitude.

[1]  Kim Lajoie,et al.  Neurons in area 5 of the posterior parietal cortex in the cat contribute to interlimb coordination during visually guided locomotion: a role in working memory. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  Amy J Bastian,et al.  Two ways to save a newly learned motor pattern. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  Richard B. Ivry,et al.  Characteristics of Implicit Sensorimotor Adaptation Revealed by Task-irrelevant Clamped Feedback , 2017, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[4]  S. M. Morton,et al.  Cerebellar Contributions to Locomotor Adaptations during Splitbelt Treadmill Walking , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  Carey L. Holleran,et al.  Clinical Practice Guideline to Improve Locomotor Function Following Chronic Stroke, Incomplete Spinal Cord Injury, and Brain Injury , 2020, Journal of neurologic physical therapy : JNPT.

[6]  Seung-Jae Kim,et al.  Effects of Visual Feedback Distortion on Gait Adaptation: Comparison of Implicit Visual Distortion Versus Conscious Modulation on Retention of Motor Learning , 2015, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[7]  A. Haith,et al.  Unlearning versus savings in visuomotor adaptation: comparing effects of washout, passage of time, and removal of errors on motor memory , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[8]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Relevance of error: what drives motor adaptation? , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  D. Popa,et al.  Functional Role of the Cerebellum in Gamma-Band Synchronization of the Sensory and Motor Cortices , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  Shawn M O'Connor,et al.  Foot Placement Relies on State Estimation during Visually Guided Walking 1 2 , 2022 .

[11]  Dagmar Sternad,et al.  Treadmill vs. overground walking: different response to physical interaction. , 2017, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  Kristan A. Leech,et al.  Feasibility of Focused Stepping Practice During Inpatient Rehabilitation Poststroke and Potential Contributions to Mobility Outcomes , 2015, Neurorehabilitation and neural repair.

[13]  M. Hallett,et al.  Rapid plasticity of human cortical movement representation induced by practice. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  S. M. Morton,et al.  A locomotor learning paradigm using distorted visual feedback elicits strategic learning. , 2018, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  Amy J Bastian,et al.  Creating flexible motor memories in human walking , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[16]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Estimating the sources of motor errors for adaptation and generalization , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  Amy J Bastian,et al.  Motor Adaptation Training for Faster Relearning , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  Jane L. Woodward,et al.  Variable Intensive Early Walking Poststroke (VIEWS) , 2016, Neurorehabilitation and neural repair.

[19]  Sarah E. Criscimagna-Hemminger,et al.  Contributions of the motor cortex to adaptive control of reaching depend on the perturbation schedule. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[20]  Amy J Bastian,et al.  Blocking trial-by-trial error correction does not interfere with motor learning in human walking. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  Richard B. Ivry,et al.  Flexible Cognitive Strategies during Motor Learning , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[22]  R. Ivry,et al.  Delayed feedback during sensorimotor learning selectively disrupts adaptation but not strategy use. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  J. Krakauer,et al.  An Implicit Plan Overrides an Explicit Strategy during Visuomotor Adaptation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  Amy J Bastian,et al.  A dual-learning paradigm can simultaneously train multiple characteristics of walking. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  How Each Movement Changes the Next: An Experimental and Theoretical Study of Fast Adaptive Priors in Reaching , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Sensory prediction errors drive cerebellum-dependent adaptation of reaching. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  V. Dietz,et al.  Contribution of feedback and feedforward strategies to locomotor adaptations. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  Richard B Ivry,et al.  The influence of task outcome on implicit motor learning , 2018, eLife.

[29]  Trevor Drew,et al.  Motor cortical regulation of sparse synergies provides a framework for the flexible control of precision walking , 2013, Front. Comput. Neurosci..

[30]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Throwing while looking through prisms. I. Focal olivocerebellar lesions impair adaptation. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  Kendra M. Cherry-Allen,et al.  A Dual-Learning Paradigm Simultaneously Improves Multiple Features of Gait Post-Stroke , 2018, Neurorehabilitation and neural repair.

[32]  G. Torres-Oviedo,et al.  Corrective Muscle Activity Reveals Subject-Specific Sensorimotor Recalibration , 2019, eNeuro.

[33]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Internal models in the cerebellum , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[34]  W. David Hairston,et al.  Human electrocortical dynamics while stepping over obstacles , 2019, Scientific Reports.

[35]  Hermano Igo Krebs,et al.  Effects of implicit visual feedback distortion on human gait , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  Olivier White,et al.  Use-Dependent and Error-Based Learning of Motor Behaviors , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  C. Hass,et al.  Comparing aftereffects after split-belt treadmill walking and unilateral stepping. , 2014, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[38]  D. Marple-Horvat,et al.  Rhythmic neuronal activity in the lateral cerebellum of the cat during visually guided stepping , 1999, The Journal of physiology.

[39]  Michael D Lewek,et al.  The role of movement errors in modifying spatiotemporal gait asymmetry post stroke: a randomized controlled trial , 2018, Clinical rehabilitation.

[40]  Firas Mawase,et al.  Kinetic adaptation during locomotion on a split-belt treadmill. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  Krista M Bond,et al.  Flexible explicit but rigid implicit learning in a visuomotor adaptation task. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[43]  Sheng-Che Yen,et al.  Using swing resistance and assistance to improve gait symmetry in individuals post-stroke. , 2015, Human movement science.

[44]  Hannah J. Block,et al.  Interlimb coordination during locomotion: what can be adapted and stored? , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  S. M. Morton,et al.  A locomotor adaptation including explicit knowledge and removal of postadaptation errors induces complete 24-hour retention. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  John W. Krakauer,et al.  Independent learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of reaching , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[47]  Hyunglae Lee,et al.  Gait symmetric adaptation: Comparing effects of implicit visual distortion versus split-belt treadmill on aftereffects of adapted step length symmetry. , 2019, Human movement science.

[48]  Welber Marinovic,et al.  Task Errors Drive Memories That Improve Sensorimotor Adaptation , 2020, The Journal of Neuroscience.