Ontogeny of collective behavior reveals a simple attraction rule

Significance Different interaction rules among animals can produce patterns of collective motion similar to those observed in bird flocks or fish schools. To help distinguish which rules are implemented in animal collectives, we studied the birth of the interaction rule in zebrafish during development from hatching to the juvenile stage. We used newly developed machine vision algorithms to track each animal in a group without mistakes. A weak attraction starts after hatching and gets stronger every day during development. Attraction consists in each larva moving toward one other larva chosen effectively at random and then switching to another one. This rule, simply by statistics, makes each individual move to regions of high density of individuals to produce collective motion. The striking patterns of collective animal behavior, including ant trails, bird flocks, and fish schools, can result from local interactions among animals without centralized control. Several of these rules of interaction have been proposed, but it has proven difficult to discriminate which ones are implemented in nature. As a method to better discriminate among interaction rules, we propose to follow the slow birth of a rule of interaction during animal development. Specifically, we followed the development of zebrafish, Danio rerio, and found that larvae turn toward each other from 7 days postfertilization and increase the intensity of interactions until 3 weeks. This developmental dataset allows testing the parameter-free predictions of a simple rule in which animals attract each other part of the time, with attraction defined as turning toward another animal chosen at random. This rule makes each individual likely move to a high density of conspecifics, and moving groups naturally emerge. Development of attraction strength corresponds to an increase in the time spent in attraction behavior. Adults were found to follow the same attraction rule, suggesting a potential significance for adults of other species.

[1]  Yoshinobu Inada,et al.  Order and flexibility in the motion of fish schools. , 2002, Journal of theoretical biology.

[2]  R. Gerlai,et al.  Maturation of shoaling behavior is accompanied by changes in the dopaminergic and serotoninergic systems in zebrafish. , 2012, Developmental psychobiology.

[3]  D. Parichy,et al.  Zebrafish in the wild: a review of natural history and new notes from the field. , 2007, Zebrafish.

[4]  W. Hamilton Geometry for the selfish herd. , 1971, Journal of theoretical biology.

[5]  I. Couzin,et al.  Inferring the structure and dynamics of interactions in schooling fish , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[6]  Jens Krause,et al.  The mechanism of aggregation behaviour in fish shoals: individuals minimize approach time to neighbours , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[7]  D. Sumpter,et al.  Inferring the rules of interaction of shoaling fish , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[8]  A. Pérez-Escudero,et al.  Collective Animal Behavior from Bayesian Estimation and Probability Matching , 2011 .

[9]  Daniel W Franks,et al.  Limited interactions in flocks: relating model simulations to empirical data , 2011, Journal of The Royal Society Interface.

[10]  Leah Edelstein-Keshet,et al.  Inferring individual rules from collective behavior , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[11]  D. Sumpter,et al.  Fast and accurate decisions through collective vigilance in fish shoals , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[12]  Florian Engert,et al.  Neural Control and Modulation of Swimming Speed in the Larval Zebrafish , 2014, Neuron.

[13]  Drew N. Robson,et al.  Brain-wide neuronal dynamics during motor adaptation in zebrafish , 2012, Nature.

[14]  I. Couzin,et al.  Collective memory and spatial sorting in animal groups. , 2002, Journal of theoretical biology.

[15]  T. Vicsek,et al.  Hierarchical group dynamics in pigeon flocks , 2010, Nature.

[16]  Paul J. B. Hart,et al.  Quorum decision-making facilitates information transfer in fish shoals , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[17]  Guy Theraulaz,et al.  Modeling Collective Animal Behavior with a Cognitive Perspective: A Methodological Framework , 2012, PloS one.

[18]  Noam Miller,et al.  Quantification of shoaling behaviour in zebrafish (Danio rerio) , 2007, Behavioural Brain Research.

[19]  I. Couzin,et al.  Shared decision-making drives collective movement in wild baboons , 2015, Science.

[20]  Andrea Cavagna,et al.  Collective Behaviour without Collective Order in Wild Swarms of Midges , 2013, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[21]  G. Parisi,et al.  Interaction ruling animal collective behavior depends on topological rather than metric distance: Evidence from a field study , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[22]  David J. T. Sumpter,et al.  Individual Rules for Trail Pattern Formation in Argentine Ants (Linepithema humile) , 2012, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[23]  A. Pérez-Escudero,et al.  A common rule for decision making in animal collectives across species , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[24]  Steven V. Viscido,et al.  Simulated evolution of selfish herd behavior. , 2005, Journal of theoretical biology.

[25]  Steven V. Viscido,et al.  The effect of population size and number of influential neighbors on the emergent properties of fish schools , 2005 .

[26]  D. Sumpter,et al.  The statistical mechanics of schooling fish captures their interactions , 2015, 1508.07708.

[27]  D. Sumpter Collective Animal Behavior , 2010 .

[28]  E. Schuman,et al.  Protein Synthesis-Dependent Associative Long-Term Memory in Larval Zebrafish , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  M. Ryan,et al.  Learned Social Preference in Zebrafish , 2004, Current Biology.

[30]  Steven V. Viscido,et al.  Individual behavior and emergent properties of fish schools: a comparison of observation and theory , 2004 .

[31]  Guy Theraulaz,et al.  From behavioural analyses to models of collective motion in fish schools , 2012, Interface Focus.

[32]  A. Pérez-Escudero,et al.  idTracker: tracking individuals in a group by automatic identification of unmarked animals , 2014, Nature Methods.

[33]  A. Czirók,et al.  Collective Motion , 1999, physics/9902023.

[34]  Kristen E. Severi,et al.  Control of visually guided behavior by distinct populations of spinal projection neurons , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[35]  G D Ruxton,et al.  Fish shoal composition: mechanisms and constraints , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[36]  Steven V. Viscido,et al.  The dilemma of the selfish herd: the search for a realistic movement rule. , 2002, Journal of theoretical biology.

[37]  Jens Krause,et al.  Shoal choice in zebrafish, Danio rerio: the influence of shoal size and activity , 2001, Animal Behaviour.

[38]  Vicsek,et al.  Novel type of phase transition in a system of self-driven particles. , 1995, Physical review letters.

[39]  D. Sumpter,et al.  Quorum Decision-Making in Foraging Fish Shoals , 2012, PloS one.

[40]  I. Couzin,et al.  Effective leadership and decision-making in animal groups on the move , 2005, Nature.

[41]  Gonçalo Lopes,et al.  Development of social behavior in young zebrafish , 2015, Front. Neural Circuits.

[42]  I. Couzin,et al.  Mechanisms underlying shoal composition in the Trinidadian guppy, Poecilia reticulata , 2003 .

[43]  L. Morrell,et al.  ‘Selfish herds’ of guppies follow complex movement rules, but not when information is limited , 2015, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.