Perceiving Affordances for Aperture Crossing for the Person-Plus-Object System

In 3 experiments we investigated perception of affordances for aperture crossing for the person-plus-object system. Perceivers wielded occluded hand-held objects and determined whether they would be able to carry those objects through an aperture of a particular size. The results suggest (a) perceivers are sensitive to this affordance both when the object is wielded but not visible and when the object is visible but not wielded, (b) perception of affordances for aperture crossing while carrying an object is constrained by the same higher order inertial variable that constrains perception of extent of a hand-held wielded object (Iij ), and (c) perceivers are sensitive to this affordance both when the shoulder width of the participant contributes to the width of the person-plus-object system and when it does not. In general, perceivers treat wielded objects as an extension of their bodies and are sensitive to affordances for the person-plus-object system.

[1]  E. Gibson,et al.  An Ecological Approach to Perceptual Learning and Development , 2000 .

[2]  M T Turvey,et al.  Perceiving the width and height of a hand-held object by dynamic touch. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  Geoffrey P. Bingham,et al.  Perceiving the size of trees: form as information about scale , 1993 .

[4]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[5]  M. Turvey,et al.  Ecological foundations of cognition. I: Symmetry and specificity of animal-environment systems , 1999 .

[6]  Raoul M Bongers,et al.  Variations of Tool and Task Characteristics Reveal That Tool-Use Postures Are Anticipated , 2004, Journal of motor behavior.

[7]  David B. Miller,et al.  Nested reciprocities: the organism-environment system in perception-action and development. , 2003, Developmental psychobiology.

[8]  E. Reed Encountering the world: Toward an ecological psychology. , 1997 .

[9]  D. M. Jacobs,et al.  On the Apparent Paradox of Learning and Realism , 2002 .

[10]  Robert B. Suter Trichobothrial mediation of an aquatic escape response: Directional jumps by the fishing spider, Dolomedes triton, foil frog attacks , 2003 .

[11]  M. Turvey Dynamic touch. , 1996, The American psychologist.

[12]  L. S. Mark,et al.  Eyeheight-scaled information about affordances: a study of sitting and stair climbing. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  N. Schenker,et al.  Overlapping confidence intervals or standard error intervals: What do they mean in terms of statistical significance? , 2003, Journal of insect science.

[14]  M. Turvey,et al.  Somatosensory attunement to the rigid body laws , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  Carl Anderson,et al.  The extended organism: The physiology of animal-built structures , 2000, Complex..

[16]  W. Warren,et al.  Visual guidance of walking through apertures: body-scaled information for affordances. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  D. M. Jacobs,et al.  Learning to visually perceive the relative mass of colliding balls in globally and locally constrained task ecologies. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  M. Turvey Affordances and Prospective Control: An Outline of the Ontology , 1992 .

[19]  D. M. Jacobs,et al.  Learning to perceive the relative mass of colliding balls: The effects of ratio scaling and feedback , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[20]  G J Savelsbergh,et al.  Locomoting through apertures of different width: a study of children with cerebral palsy. , 1998, Pediatric rehabilitation.

[21]  Christopher C. Pagano,et al.  Eigenvectors of the Inertia Tensor and Perceiving the Orientations of Limbs and Objects , 1998 .

[22]  C. Michaels,et al.  Higher order and lower order variables in the visual perception of relative pulling force. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[23]  Michael S. Gordon,et al.  Perception of Sound-Obstructing Surfaces Using Body-Scaled Judgments , 2004 .

[24]  J. Gaddum Probit Analysis , 1948, Nature.

[25]  M. Turvey,et al.  Eigenvalues of the inertia tensor and exteroception by the “muscular sense” , 1994, Neuroscience.

[26]  William R. Wiener,et al.  The Auditory Skills Necessary for Echolocation: A New Explanation , 1996 .

[27]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Physics and Psychology of the Muscle Sense , 2004 .

[28]  W. Epstein,et al.  Perception of space and motion , 1995 .

[29]  Claudia Carello,et al.  Haptic and Visual Perception of an Affordance for Upright Posture , 1994 .

[30]  T. Lombardo,et al.  The reciprocity of perceiver and environment : the evolution of James J. Gibson's ecological psychology , 1987 .

[31]  Jeffrey B. Wagman,et al.  Affordances and Inertial Constraints on Tool Use , 2001 .

[32]  James A. Balliett,et al.  What an actor must do in order to perceive the affordance for sitting. , 1990 .

[33]  Illusions and Knowing What Is Real , 1992 .

[34]  A. Noē Direct Perception , 2022 .

[35]  E. Gibson Principles of Perceptual Learning and Development , 1969 .

[36]  Jeffrey B Wagman,et al.  Haptically creating affordances: the user-tool interface. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[37]  M. Turvey,et al.  Ecological laws of perceiving and acting: In reply to Fodor and Pylyshyn (1981) , 1981, Cognition.

[38]  Claudia Carello,et al.  Perceiving Affordances by Dynamic Touch: Hints From the Control of Movement , 2004 .

[39]  G. Burton,et al.  Nonvisual judgment of the crossability of path gaps. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  A. Smitsman,et al.  Affordances, perceptual complexity, and the development of tool use. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[41]  W H Warren,et al.  Perceiving affordances: visual guidance of stair climbing. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[42]  Takahiro Higuchi,et al.  Visual estimation of spatial requirements for locomotion in novice wheelchair users. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[43]  J. Elser,et al.  A microfluorometric method for quantifying RNA and DNA in terrestrial insects , 2003, Journal of insect science.

[44]  S. Runeson The distorted room illusion, equivalent configurations, and the specificity of static optic arrays. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[45]  A. Smitsman,et al.  The development of tool use: Changing boundaries between organism and environment. , 1997 .

[46]  Naoya Hirose,et al.  The Process of Adaptation to Perceiving New Action Capabilities , 2001 .

[47]  A. Berti,et al.  When Far Becomes Near: Remapping of Space by Tool Use , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[48]  M T Turvey,et al.  Role of the inertia tensor in perceiving object orientation by dynamic touch. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[49]  M T Turvey,et al.  Attunement, Calibration, and Exploration in Fast Haptic Perceptual Learning , 2001, Journal of motor behavior.

[50]  G. Bingham,et al.  Hefting for a maximum distance throw: a smart perceptual mechanism. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[51]  Rick L. Jenison,et al.  Auditory Spatial Layout , 1995 .

[52]  M. Turvey,et al.  Visually perceiving what is reachable. , 1989 .

[53]  Jeffrey B Wagman,et al.  Chosen striking location and the user-tool-environment system. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.