More than just perception–action recalibration: Walking through a virtual environment causes rescaling of perceived space

Egocentric distances in virtual environments are commonly underperceived by up to 50 % of the intended distance. However, a brief period of interaction in which participants walk through the virtual environment while receiving visual feedback can dramatically improve distance judgments. Two experiments were designed to explore whether the increase in postinteraction distance judgments is due to perception–action recalibration or the rescaling of perceived space. Perception–action recalibration as a result of walking interaction should only affect action-specific distance judgments, whereas rescaling of perceived space should affect all distance judgments based on the rescaled percept. Participants made blind-walking distance judgments and verbal size judgments in response to objects in a virtual environment before and after interacting with the environment through either walking (Experiment 1) or reaching (Experiment 2). Size judgments were used to infer perceived distance under the assumption of size–distance invariance, and these served as an implicit measure of perceived distance. Preinteraction walking and size-based distance judgments indicated an underperception of egocentric distance, whereas postinteraction walking and size-based distance judgments both increased as a result of the walking interaction, indicating that walking through the virtual environment with continuous visual feedback caused rescaling of the perceived space. However, interaction with the virtual environment through reaching had no effect on either type of distance judgment, indicating that physical translation through the virtual environment may be necessary for a rescaling of perceived space. Furthermore, the size-based distance and walking distance judgments were highly correlated, even across changes in perceived distance, providing support for the size–distance invariance hypothesis.

[1]  A. S. Gilinsky Perceived size and distance in visual space. , 1951, Psychological review.

[2]  E. Gibson,et al.  The effect of training on absolute estimation of distance over the ground. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[3]  W. Epstein,et al.  The current status of the size-distance hypotheses. , 1961, Psychological bulletin.

[4]  W C Gogel,et al.  The sensing of retinal size. , 1969, Vision research.

[5]  William Epstein,et al.  Size and distance judgments under reduced conditions of viewing , 1969 .

[6]  Walter C. Gogel,et al.  The validity of the size-distance invariance hypothesis with cue reduction , 1971 .

[7]  Walter C. Gogel,et al.  Absolute motion parallax and the specific distance tendency , 1973 .

[8]  Tadasu Oyama,et al.  Perceived size and perceived distance in stereoscopic vision and an analysis of their causal relations , 1974 .

[9]  John M Foley,et al.  Effect of Distance Information and Range on Two Indices of Visually Perceived Distance , 1977, Perception.

[10]  O. Sabouraud [Space perception]. , 1978, Revue d'oto-neuro-ophtalmologie.

[11]  D. Ja,et al.  Scales for perceived egocentric distance in a large open field: comparison of three psychophysical methods. , 1985 .

[12]  J M Loomis,et al.  Agreement between indirect measures of perceived distance , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.

[13]  L. Kaufman,et al.  Handbook of perception and human performance , 1986 .

[14]  D W Eby,et al.  A study of visually directed throwing in the presence of multiple distance cues , 1987, Perception & psychophysics.

[15]  J. Rieser,et al.  Visual Perception and the Guidance of Locomotion without Vision to Previously Seen Targets , 1990, Perception.

[16]  J. Marshall,et al.  Left neglect for near but not far space in man , 1991, Nature.

[17]  J. Loomis,et al.  Visual space perception and visually directed action. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  Anne E. Garing,et al.  Calibration of human locomotion and models of perceptual-motor organization. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  J M Loomis,et al.  Visually perceived location is an invariant in the control of action , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[20]  J. Philbeck,et al.  Comparison of two indicators of perceived egocentric distance under full-cue and reduced-cue conditions , 1997 .

[21]  G P Bingham,et al.  The necessity of a perception-action approach to definite distance perception: monocular distance perception to guide reaching. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  Wallace J. Sadowski,et al.  Nonvisually Guided Locomotion to a Previously Viewed Target in Real and Virtual Environments , 1998, Hum. Factors.

[23]  Zijiang J. He,et al.  Terrain influences the accurate judgement of distance , 1998, Nature.

[24]  T Landis,et al.  Near and far visual space in unilateral neglect , 1998, Annals of neurology.

[25]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Assessing auditory distance perception using perceptually directed action , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.

[26]  F. Previc The neuropsychology of 3-D space. , 1998, Psychological bulletin.

[27]  James R. Tresilian,et al.  Increasing confidence in vergence as a cue to distance , 1999, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[28]  Zijiang J. He,et al.  Distance determined by the angular declination below the horizon , 2001, Nature.

[29]  Peter Willemsen,et al.  Evaluating space perception in NPR immersive environments , 2002, NPAR '02.

[30]  Rob Withagen,et al.  The Calibration of Walking Transfers to Crawling: Are Action Systems Calibrated? , 2002 .

[31]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Visual perception of egocentric distance in real and virtual environments. , 2003 .

[32]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Limited Field of View of Head-Mounted Displays Is Not the Cause of Distance Underestimation in Virtual Environments , 2004, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[33]  Hugo Bruggeman,et al.  Learning to throw on a rotating carousel: recalibration based on limb dynamics and projectile kinematics , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  Zijiang J. He,et al.  Perceiving distance accurately by a directional process of integrating ground information , 2004, Nature.

[35]  Stuart Anstis,et al.  Aftereffects from jogging , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  Peter Willemsen,et al.  Does the Quality of the Computer Graphics Matter when Judging Distances in Visually Immersive Environments? , 2004, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[37]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Perception of Shared Visual Space: Establishing Common Ground in Real and Virtual Environments , 2004, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[38]  B. Mohler,et al.  Calibration of Locomotion due to Visual Motion in a Treadmill-based Virtual Environment , 2005 .

[39]  Adam R. Richardson,et al.  The effect of feedback training on distance estimation in virtual environments , 2005 .

[40]  Frank H. Durgin,et al.  Distance Perception and the Visual Horizon in Head-Mounted Displays , 2005, TAP.

[41]  W. Epstein,et al.  Tool use affects perceived distance, but only when you intend to use it. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[42]  Laura F. Fox,et al.  Self-motion perception during locomotor recalibration: more than meets the eye. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[43]  Mark Wexler,et al.  Depth perception by the active observer , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[44]  Jack M Loomis,et al.  Does energy expenditure affect the perception of egocentric distance? A failure to replicate experiment 1 of Proffitt, Stefanucci, Banton, and Epstein (2003). , 2010, The Spanish journal of psychology.

[45]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Does Energy Expenditure Affect the Perception of Egocentric Distance? A Failure to Replicate Experiment 1 of Proffitt, Stefanucci, Banton, and Epstein (2003) , 2006, The Spanish Journal of Psychology.

[46]  Betty J. Mohler,et al.  The influence of feedback on egocentric distance judgments in real and virtual environments , 2006, APGV '06.

[47]  Timothy P. McNamara,et al.  Distance estimation in virtual and real environments using bisection , 2007, APGV.

[48]  Betty J. Mohler,et al.  Calibration of locomotion resulting from visual motion in a treadmill-based virtual environment , 2007, TAP.

[49]  David Waller,et al.  Interaction With an Immersive Virtual Environment Corrects Users' Distance Estimates , 2007, Hum. Factors.

[50]  Peter Willemsen,et al.  Effects of Stereo Viewing Conditions on Distance Perception in Virtual Environments , 2008, PRESENCE: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments.

[51]  David Waller,et al.  Correcting distance estimates by interacting with immersive virtual environments: effects of task and available sensory information. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[52]  William B. Thompson,et al.  HMD calibration and its effects on distance judgments , 2008, APGV '08.

[53]  Christine J. Ziemer,et al.  Estimating distance in real and virtual environments: Does order make a difference? , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[54]  Klaus H. Hinrichs,et al.  Transitional environments enhance distance perception in immersive virtual reality systems , 2009, APGV '09.

[55]  Peter Willemsen,et al.  The effects of head-mounted display mechanical properties and field of view on distance judgments in virtual environments , 2009, TAP.

[56]  Yann Coello,et al.  Motor representations and the perception of space: perceptual judgments of the boundary of action space , 2010 .

[57]  Nicholas A. Giudice,et al.  Representing 3D Space in Working Memory: Spatial Images from Vision, Hearing, Touch, and Language , 2013 .

[58]  Ágoston Török,et al.  Reference frames in virtual spatial navigation are viewpoint dependent , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[59]  Alessandro Soranzo,et al.  The Use of Virtual Reality in Psychology: A Case Study in Visual Perception , 2015, Comput. Math. Methods Medicine.