Neural Connectivity in Syntactic Movement Processing

Linguistic theory suggests non-canonical sentences subvert the dominant agent-verb-theme order in English via displacement of sentence constituents to argument (NP-movement) or non-argument positions (wh-movement). Both processes have been associated with the left inferior frontal gyrus and posterior superior temporal gyrus, but differences in neural activity and connectivity between movement types have not been investigated. In the current study, functional magnetic resonance imaging data were acquired from 21 adult participants during an auditory sentence-picture verification task using passive and active sentences contrasted to isolate NP-movement, and object- and subject-cleft sentences contrasted to isolate wh-movement. Then, functional magnetic resonance imaging data from regions common to both movement types were entered into a dynamic causal modeling analysis to examine effective connectivity for wh-movement and NP-movement. Results showed greater left inferior frontal gyrus activation for Wh > NP-movement, but no activation for NP > Wh-movement. Both types of movement elicited activity in the opercular part of the left inferior frontal gyrus, left posterior superior temporal gyrus, and left medial superior frontal gyrus. The dynamic causal modeling analyses indicated that neither movement type significantly modulated the connection from the left inferior frontal gyrus to the left posterior superior temporal gyrus, nor vice-versa, suggesting no connectivity differences between wh- and NP-movement. These findings support the idea that increased complexity of wh-structures, compared to sentences with NP-movement, requires greater engagement of cognitive resources via increased neural activity in the left inferior frontal gyrus, but both movement types engage similar neural networks.

[1]  Lorraine K Tyler,et al.  Language-related domain-specific and domain-general systems in the human brain , 2018, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[2]  John C. Trueswell,et al.  Relational vs. attributive interpretation of nominal compounds differentially engages angular gyrus and anterior temporal lobe , 2017, Brain and Language.

[3]  L. Tyler,et al.  Robust Resilience of the Frontotemporal Syntax System to Aging , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  Gregory Hickok,et al.  ‘Syntactic Perturbation’ During Production Activates the Right IFG, but not Broca’s Area or the ATL , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[5]  Alexander Kogan,et al.  The Northwestern University Neuroimaging Data Archive (NUNDA) , 2016, NeuroImage.

[6]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Can parametric statistical methods be trusted for fMRI based group studies? , 2015, 1511.01863.

[7]  John C. Trueswell,et al.  Compositionality and the angular gyrus: A multi-voxel similarity analysis of the semantic composition of nouns and verbs , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  Luigi Cattaneo,et al.  Thematic role assignment in the posterior parietal cortex: A TMS study , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  David Poeppel,et al.  The neural bases of taxonomic and thematic conceptual relations: An MEG study , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  Junwei Zhu,et al.  Differences in grammatical processing strategies for active and passive sentences: An fMRI study , 2015, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[11]  Pascale Tremblay,et al.  The Language Connectome , 2014, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[12]  Jeremy I. Skipper Echoes of the spoken past: how auditory cortex hears context during speech perception , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[13]  Aya Meltzer-Asscher,et al.  Neurocognitive mechanisms of verb argument structure processing , 2014 .

[14]  J. Martino,et al.  Subcortical anatomy of the lateral association fascicles of the brain: A review , 2014, Clinical anatomy.

[15]  Naama Friedmann,et al.  The processing of different syntactic structures: fMRI investigation of the linguistic distinction between wh-movement and verb movement , 2014, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[16]  A. Friederici,et al.  Hierarchical functional connectivity between the core language system and the working memory system , 2013, Cortex.

[17]  Jennifer E. Mack,et al.  Neural Correlates of Processing Passive Sentences , 2013, Brain sciences.

[18]  M. Catani,et al.  A novel frontal pathway underlies verbal fluency in primary progressive aphasia. , 2013, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[19]  Line Burholt Kristensen,et al.  The interface between language and attention: prosodic focus marking recruits a general attention network in spoken language comprehension. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[20]  M. Schlesewsky,et al.  Reconciling time, space and function: A new dorsal–ventral stream model of sentence comprehension , 2013, Brain and Language.

[21]  K. Amunts,et al.  Processing noncanonical sentences in broca's region: reflections of movement distance and type. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[22]  Peter Hagoort,et al.  Syntactic priming and the lexical boost effect during sentence production and sentence comprehension: An fMRI study , 2013, Brain and Language.

[23]  Patti Adank,et al.  Design choices in imaging speech comprehension: An Activation Likelihood Estimation (ALE) meta-analysis , 2012, NeuroImage.

[24]  Patti Adank,et al.  The neural bases of difficult speech comprehension and speech production: Two Activation Likelihood Estimation (ALE) meta-analyses , 2012, Brain and Language.

[25]  Norihiro Sadato,et al.  The neural substrates associated with attentional resources and difficulty of concurrent processing of the two verbal tasks , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  A. Friederici The cortical language circuit: from auditory perception to sentence comprehension , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[27]  Ina Bornkessel-Schlesewsky,et al.  Prominence vs. aboutness in sequencing: A functional distinction within the left inferior frontal gyrus , 2012, Brain and Language.

[28]  Jens Timmer,et al.  Network modulation during complex syntactic processing , 2012, NeuroImage.

[29]  Angela D Friederici,et al.  Who was the agent? The neural correlates of reanalysis processes during sentence comprehension , 2011, Human brain mapping.

[30]  A. Friederici The brain basis of language processing: from structure to function. , 2011, Physiological reviews.

[31]  Gregory Hickok,et al.  The Role of Broca's Area in Sentence Comprehension , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  Todd B. Parrish,et al.  Neural plasticity and treatment-induced recovery of sentence processing in agrammatism , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  Cynthia K. Thompson,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Verb Argument Structure Processing in Agrammatic Aphasic and Healthy Age-matched Listeners , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  Peter Zeidman,et al.  Identifying Abnormal Connectivity in Patients Using Dynamic Causal Modeling of fMRI Responses , 2010, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[35]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Ten simple rules for dynamic causal modeling , 2010, NeuroImage.

[36]  C. Thompson,et al.  Automatic processing of wh- and NP-movement in agrammatic aphasia: Evidence from eyetracking , 2009, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[37]  Xiaolin Zhou,et al.  Conflict control during sentence comprehension: fMRI evidence , 2009, NeuroImage.

[38]  Monica Baciu,et al.  The sensory-motor specificity of taxonomic and thematic conceptual relations: A behavioral and fMRI study , 2009, NeuroImage.

[39]  Y. Grodzinsky,et al.  The battle for Broca’s region , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[40]  A. Turken,et al.  Left inferior frontal gyrus is critical for response inhibition , 2008, BMC Neuroscience.

[41]  Ryuta Kinno,et al.  Neural correlates of noncanonical syntactic processing revealed by a picture‐sentence matching task , 2008, Human brain mapping.

[42]  David Caplan,et al.  Syntactic and Thematic Constraint Effects on Blood Oxygenation Level Dependent Signal Correlates of Comprehension of Relative Clauses , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[43]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dynamic causal modelling for fMRI: A two-state model , 2008, NeuroImage.

[44]  Shigeru Sato,et al.  Is Broca's area involved in the processing of passive sentences? An event-related fMRI study , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[45]  Abraham Z. Snyder,et al.  A default mode of brain function: A brief history of an evolving idea , 2007, NeuroImage.

[46]  C. Thompson,et al.  Real-time comprehension of wh- movement in aphasia: Evidence from eyetracking while listening , 2007, Brain and Language.

[47]  Brian Everitt,et al.  A systematic review and quantitative appraisal of fMRI studies of verbal fluency: Role of the left inferior frontal gyrus , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[48]  Kristina M. Visscher,et al.  A Core System for the Implementation of Task Sets , 2006, Neuron.

[49]  Shigeru Sato,et al.  Cortical activation in the processing of passive sentences in L1 and L2: An fMRI study , 2006, NeuroImage.

[50]  L. Cohen,et al.  The role of the supplementary motor area (SMA) in word production , 2006, Brain Research.

[51]  P. Hagoort On Broca, brain, and binding: a new framework , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[52]  Angela D. Friederici,et al.  Who did what to whom? The neural basis of argument hierarchies during language comprehension , 2005, NeuroImage.

[53]  Yosef Grodzinsky,et al.  Neural correlates of syntactic movement: converging evidence from two fMRI experiments , 2004, NeuroImage.

[54]  Pratik Mukherjee,et al.  Subcortical pathways serving cortical language sites: initial experience with diffusion tensor imaging fiber tracking combined with intraoperative language mapping , 2004, NeuroImage.

[55]  C. Thompson,et al.  The resolution and recovery of filler-gap dependencies in aphasia: Evidence from on-line anomaly detection , 2004, Brain and Language.

[56]  T. Hendler,et al.  The Neural Reality of Syntactic Transformations , 2003, Psychological science.

[57]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dynamic causal modelling , 2003, NeuroImage.

[58]  J. Grafman,et al.  Dissociating the roles of the rostral anterior cingulate and the lateral prefrontal cortices in performing two tasks simultaneously or successively. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[59]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[60]  Stéphane Lehéricy,et al.  Distinct prefrontal activations in processing sequence at the sentence and script level: An fMRI study , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[61]  G. Waters,et al.  PET Studies of Syntactic Processing with Auditory Sentence Presentation , 1999, NeuroImage.

[62]  Lewis P. Shapiro,et al.  Prosody and the processing of filler-gap sentences , 1994, Journal of psycholinguistic research.

[63]  V. Fromkin,et al.  Comprehension and Acceptability Judgments in Agrammatism: Disruptions in the Syntax of Referential Dependency , 1993, Brain and Language.

[64]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Hierarchical organization and functional streams in the visual cortex , 1983, Trends in Neurosciences.

[65]  Ina Bornkessel-Schlesewsky,et al.  The Cambridge Handbook of Biolinguistics: Computational primitives in syntax and possible brain correlates , 2013 .

[66]  Christos Salis,et al.  Comprehension of wh-questions in agrammatism: a single-case study , 2005 .

[67]  Ming-Wei Lee,et al.  Another Look at the Role of Empty Categories in Sentence Processing (and Grammar) , 2004, Journal of psycholinguistic research.