Computational phenotyping of brain-behavior dynamics underlying approach-avoidance conflict in major depressive disorder

Adaptive behavior requires balancing approach and avoidance based on the rewarding and aversive consequences of actions. Imbalances in this evaluation are thought to characterize mood disorders such as major depressive disorder (MDD). We present a novel application of the drift diffusion model (DDM) suited to quantify how offers of reward and aversiveness, and neural correlates thereof, are dynamically integrated to form decisions, and how such processes are altered in MDD. Hierarchical parameter estimation from the DDM demonstrated that the MDD group differed in three distinct reward-related parameters driving approach-based decision making. First, MDD was associated with reduced reward sensitivity, measured as the impact of offered reward on evidence accumulation. Notably, this effect was replicated in a follow-up study. Second, the MDD group showed lower starting point bias towards approaching offers. Third, this starting point was influenced in opposite directions by Pavlovian effects and by nucleus accumbens activity across the groups: greater accumbens activity was related to approach bias in controls but avoid bias in MDD. Cross-validation revealed that the combination of these computational biomarkers were diagnostic of patient status, with accumbens influences being particularly diagnostic. Finally, within the MDD group, reward sensitivity and nucleus accumbens parameters were differentially related to symptoms of perceived stress and depression. Collectively, these findings establish the promise of computational psychiatry approaches to dissecting approach-avoidance decision dynamics relevant for affective disorders.

[1]  M. Rinck,et al.  Biased approach-avoidance tendencies in psychopathology: A systematic review of their assessment and modification. , 2020, Clinical psychology review.

[2]  Jessica I. Määttä,et al.  Dopamine promotes cognitive effort by biasing the benefits versus costs of cognitive work , 2019, Science.

[3]  B. Forstmann,et al.  Advances in techniques for imposing reciprocity in brain-behavior relations , 2019, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[4]  John J. B. Allen,et al.  Multiple Dissociations Between Comorbid Depression and Anxiety on Reward and Punishment Processing: Evidence From Computationally Informed EEG , 2019, Computational Psychiatry.

[5]  B. Hayden Economic choice: the foraging perspective , 2018, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[6]  Camilla L. Nord,et al.  Depression is associated with enhanced aversive Pavlovian control over instrumental behaviour , 2018, Scientific Reports.

[7]  R. Dolan,et al.  Neural activity and fundamental learning, motivated by monetary loss and reward, are intact in mild to moderate major depressive disorder , 2018, PloS one.

[8]  A. Graybiel,et al.  Striatal Microstimulation Induces Persistent and Repetitive Negative Decision-Making Predicted by Striatal Beta-Band Oscillation , 2018, Neuron.

[9]  R. Bogacz,et al.  Mechanisms Underlying Decision-Making as Revealed by Deep-Brain Stimulation in Patients with Parkinson’s Disease , 2018, Current Biology.

[10]  K. Roelofs,et al.  Approach and avoidance tendencies in depression and anxiety disorders , 2017, Psychiatry Research.

[11]  Paul-Christian Bürkner,et al.  brms: An R Package for Bayesian Multilevel Models Using Stan , 2017 .

[12]  M. Frank,et al.  The drift diffusion model as the choice rule in reinforcement learning , 2017, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[13]  M. Thomason,et al.  You say ‘prefrontal cortex' and I say ‘anterior cingulate': meta-analysis of spatial overlap in amygdala-to-prefrontal connectivity and internalizing symptomology , 2016, Translational psychiatry.

[14]  R. Bogacz,et al.  Neural Correlates of Decision Thresholds in the Human Subthalamic Nucleus , 2016, Current Biology.

[15]  M. Frank,et al.  Computational psychiatry as a bridge from neuroscience to clinical applications , 2016, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  Thomas V. Wiecki,et al.  A Computational Cognitive Biomarker for Early-Stage Huntington’s Disease , 2016, PloS one.

[17]  P. Dayan,et al.  The specificity of Pavlovian regulation is associated with recovery from depression , 2016, Psychological Medicine.

[18]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Sequential Sampling Models in Cognitive Neuroscience: Advantages, Applications, and Extensions. , 2016, Annual review of psychology.

[19]  A. Graybiel,et al.  A Corticostriatal Path Targeting Striosomes Controls Decision-Making under Conflict , 2015, Cell.

[20]  Thomas V. Wiecki,et al.  Model-Based Cognitive Neuroscience Approaches to Computational Psychiatry , 2015 .

[21]  Ken-ichi Amemori,et al.  Motivation and Affective Judgments Differentially Recruit Neurons in the Primate Dorsolateral Prefrontal and Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  Martin P Paulus,et al.  Neural substrates of approach‐avoidance conflict decision‐making , 2015, Human brain mapping.

[23]  Thomas V. Wiecki,et al.  fMRI and EEG Predictors of Dynamic Decision Parameters during Human Reinforcement Learning , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  Tom Beckers,et al.  A Bayesian hierarchical diffusion model decomposition of performance in Approach–Avoidance Tasks , 2014, Cognition & emotion.

[25]  M. Frank,et al.  Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  K. Dobson,et al.  An examination of avoidance in major depression in comparison to social anxiety disorder. , 2014, Behaviour research and therapy.

[27]  D. Pizzagalli,et al.  Depression, stress, and anhedonia: toward a synthesis and integrated model. , 2014, Annual review of clinical psychology.

[28]  P. Dayan,et al.  Mapping anhedonia onto reinforcement learning: a behavioural meta-analysis , 2013, Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders.

[29]  Thomas V. Wiecki,et al.  HDDM: Hierarchical Bayesian estimation of the Drift-Diffusion Model in Python , 2013, Front. Neuroinform..

[30]  Dino J. Levy,et al.  The root of all value: a neural common currency for choice , 2012, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[31]  Raymond J. Dolan,et al.  Go and no-go learning in reward and punishment: Interactions between affect and effect , 2012, NeuroImage.

[32]  Ken-ichi Amemori,et al.  Localized Microstimulation of Primate Pregenual Cingulate Cortex Induces Negative Decision-Making , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[33]  Russell A. Poldrack,et al.  Deconvolving BOLD activation in event-related designs for multivoxel pattern classification analyses , 2012, NeuroImage.

[34]  T. Sharot The optimism bias , 2011, Current Biology.

[35]  M. Paulus,et al.  A reverse translational approach to quantify approach-avoidance conflict in humans , 2011, Behavioural Brain Research.

[36]  Thomas V. Wiecki,et al.  Subthalamic nucleus stimulation reverses mediofrontal influence over decision threshold , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[37]  A. Rangel,et al.  Multialternative drift-diffusion model predicts the relationship between visual fixations and choice in value-based decisions , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[38]  Raymond J. Dolan,et al.  Disentangling the Roles of Approach, Activation and Valence in Instrumental and Pavlovian Responding , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[39]  A. Rangel,et al.  Visual fixations and the computation and comparison of value in simple choice , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[40]  John K Kruschke,et al.  Bayesian data analysis. , 2010, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[41]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Using diffusion models to understand clinical disorders. , 2010, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[42]  Jeffrey L. Birk,et al.  Reduced caudate and nucleus accumbens response to rewards in unmedicated individuals with major depressive disorder. , 2009, The American journal of psychiatry.

[43]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  The Diffusion Decision Model: Theory and Data for Two-Choice Decision Tasks , 2008, Neural Computation.

[44]  C. Carter,et al.  Reward-related decision-making in pediatric major depressive disorder: an fMRI study. , 2006, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[45]  A. MacLeod,et al.  Approach and Avoidance Goals and Plans: Their Relationship to Anxiety and Depression , 2004, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[46]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time. , 2004, Psychological review.

[47]  M. Delgado,et al.  Dorsal striatum responses to reward and punishment: Effects of valence and magnitude manipulations , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[48]  T. Robbins,et al.  Nucleus accumbens dopamine depletion impairs both acquisition and performance of appetitive Pavlovian approach behaviour: implications for mesoaccumbens dopamine function , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.

[49]  B. Everitt,et al.  Differential Involvement of NMDA, AMPA/Kainate, and Dopamine Receptors in the Nucleus Accumbens Core in the Acquisition and Performance of Pavlovian Approach Behavior , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[50]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociation in Effects of Lesions of the Nucleus Accumbens Core and Shell on Appetitive Pavlovian Approach Behavior and the Potentiation of Conditioned Reinforcement and Locomotor Activity byd-Amphetamine , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  M. Hamilton,et al.  A Scale for the Assessment of Hedonic Tone the Snaith–Hamilton Pleasure Scale , 1995, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[52]  D. Rubin,et al.  Inference from Iterative Simulation Using Multiple Sequences , 1992 .

[53]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .

[54]  M. Hamilton A RATING SCALE FOR DEPRESSION , 1960, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[55]  P. Glimcher,et al.  Title: the Neural Representation of Subjective Value under Risk and Ambiguity 1 2 , 2009 .

[56]  P. Lang International affective picture system (IAPS) : affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual , 2005 .