Cooperative vocal control in marmoset monkeys via vocal feedback.

Humans adjust speech amplitude as a function of distance from a listener; we do so in a manner that would compensate for such distance. This ability is presumed to be the product of high-level sociocognitive skills. Nonhuman primates are thought to lack such socially related flexibility in vocal production. Using predictions from a simple arousal-based model whereby vocal feedback from a conspecific modulates the drive to produce a vocalization, we tested whether another primate exhibits this type of cooperative vocal control. We conducted a playback experiment with marmoset monkeys and simulated "far-away" and "nearby" conspecifics using contact calls that differed in sound intensity. We found that marmoset monkeys increased the amplitude of their contact calls and produced such calls with shorter response latencies toward more distant conspecifics. The same was not true in response to changing levels of background noise. To account for how simulated conspecific distance can change both the amplitude and timing of vocal responses, we developed a model that incorporates dynamic interactions between the auditory system and limbic "drive" systems. Overall, our data show that, like humans, marmoset monkeys cooperatively control the acoustics of their vocalizations according to changes in listener distance, increasing the likelihood that a conspecific will hear their call. However, we propose that such cooperative vocal control is a system property that does not necessitate any particularly advanced sociocognitive skill. At least in marmosets, this vocal control can be parsimoniously explained by the regulation of arousal states across two interacting individuals via vocal feedback.

[1]  Edward L. Bartlett,et al.  Long-lasting modulation by stimulus context in primate auditory cortex. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  Monica Maranesi,et al.  Neurons Controlling Voluntary Vocalization in the Macaque Ventral Premotor Cortex , 2011, PloS one.

[3]  D. Todt,et al.  Acoustic communication in noise: regulation of call characteristics in a New World monkey , 2004, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[4]  R. Seyfarth,et al.  Vocal development in vervet monkeys , 1986, Animal Behaviour.

[5]  Alex A. Pollen,et al.  Temporal cues in the antiphonal long-calling behaviour of cottontop tamarins , 2002, Animal Behaviour.

[6]  Sabyasachi Roy,et al.  Vocal control by the common marmoset in the presence of interfering noise , 2011, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[7]  Katherine A. Cronin,et al.  Cooperative breeders do cooperate , 2007, Behavioural Processes.

[8]  A. Winkworth,et al.  Breathing patterns during spontaneous speech. , 1995, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[9]  Drew Rendall,et al.  Social brains, simple minds: does social complexity really require cognitive complexity? , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[10]  Cory T. Miller,et al.  Sensory-motor interactions modulate a primate vocal behavior: antiphonal calling in common marmosets , 2005, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[11]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[12]  Effects of Interpersonal Distance on Children's Vocal Intensity. , 1981 .

[13]  Judith M Burkart,et al.  Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution , 2009 .

[14]  Andreas Nieder,et al.  Cognitive Control of Distinct Vocalizations in Rhesus Monkeys , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  S. Weisberg,et al.  Residuals and Influence in Regression , 1982 .

[16]  J. Kaas,et al.  Prefrontal connections of the parabelt auditory cortex in macaque monkeys , 1999, Brain Research.

[17]  Carel P. van Schaik,et al.  Cognitive consequences of cooperative breeding in primates? , 2009, Animal Cognition.

[18]  E C Healey,et al.  Effects of perceived listeners on speakers' vocal intensity. , 1997, Journal of voice : official journal of the Voice Foundation.

[19]  Christopher DiMattina,et al.  Virtual vocalization stimuli for investigating neural representations of species-specific vocalizations. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  Marc Naguib,et al.  Estimating the distance to a source of sound: mechanisms and adaptations for long-range communication , 2001, Animal Behaviour.

[21]  D. Blumstein Acoustic Communication in Insects and Anurans : Common Problems and Diverse Solutions , 2002 .

[22]  Eleanor A. McC. Gamble,et al.  Minor studies from the psychological laboratory of Wellesley College: Intensity as a criterion in estimating the distance of sounds. , 1909 .

[23]  日原 さやか,et al.  Spontaneous vocal differentiation of coo-calls for tools and food in Japanese monkeys , 2003 .

[24]  E. Briefer Vocal Expression of Emotions in Mammals: Mechanisms of Production and Evidence , 2012 .

[25]  M. Hauser,et al.  Noise‐induced vocal modulation in cotton‐top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) , 2006, American journal of primatology.

[26]  P. Narins,et al.  CALL DIVERSITY IN AN OLD WORLD TREEFROG: LEVEL DEPENDENCE AND LATENCY OF ACOUSTIC RESPONSES , 2002 .

[27]  C. Larson,et al.  Vocalization in rhesus monkeys: conditionability. , 1973, Brain research.

[28]  R. Seyfarth,et al.  Production, usage, and comprehension in animal vocalizations , 2010, Brain and Language.

[29]  M. Owren,et al.  Infant cross‐fostering between Japanese (Macaca fuscata) and rhesus macaques (M. mulatta) , 1989, American journal of primatology.

[30]  Marc D. Hauser,et al.  Tracking silence: adjusting vocal production to avoid acoustic interference , 2007, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[31]  U. Jürgens,et al.  The cingular vocalization pathway in the squirrel monkey , 1979, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  G M Siegel,et al.  Effects of distance on vocal intensity. , 1995, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[33]  Xiaoqin Wang,et al.  Sensory-motor interaction in the primate auditory cortex during self-initiated vocalizations. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Dual streams of auditory afferents target multiple domains in the primate prefrontal cortex , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[35]  Biosocial factors in dyadic communication: Sex and speaking intensity. , 1972 .

[36]  Nikos K. Logothetis,et al.  Auditory looming perception in rhesus monkeys , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[37]  B. McCowan,et al.  Amplitude of bison bellows reflects male quality, physical condition and motivation , 2008, Animal Behaviour.

[38]  U. Jürgens,et al.  On the role of the anterior cingulate cortex in phonation: A case report , 1982, Brain and Language.

[39]  Daniel H. Ashmead,et al.  Perception of the relative distances of nearby sound sources , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[40]  Cheol-Ho Jeong,et al.  Vocal effort with changing talker-to-listener distance in different acoustic environments. , 2011, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[41]  S. Sober,et al.  Vocal learning is constrained by the statistics of sensorimotor experience , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[42]  T. Paus Primate anterior cingulate cortex: Where motor control, drive and cognition interface , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[43]  M. Naguib Use of song amplitude for ranging in Carolina wrens, Thryothorus ludovicianus , 2010 .

[44]  Xiaoqin Wang,et al.  Neural Correlates of the Lombard Effect in Primate Auditory Cortex , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  Xiaoqin Wang,et al.  Comparison of auditory-vocal interactions across multiple types of vocalizations in marmoset auditory cortex. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  K. Hammerschmidt,et al.  Changes In Rhesus Macaque ‘Coo’ Vocalizations during Early Development , 2000 .

[47]  Asif A Ghazanfar,et al.  Multisensory Integration of Looming Signals by Rhesus Monkeys , 2004, Neuron.

[48]  Xiaoqin Wang,et al.  Dynamics of auditory-vocal interaction in monkey auditory cortex. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[49]  Asif A. Ghazanfar,et al.  The units of perception in the antiphonal calling behavior of cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus): playback experiments with long calls , 2001, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[50]  U. Jürgens Neuronal Control of Mammalian Vocalization, with Special Reference to the Squirrel Monkey , 1998, The Science of Nature.

[51]  Brian S. Nelson,et al.  Avian dependence on sound pressure level as an auditory distance cue , 2000, Animal Behaviour.

[52]  D. Amaral,et al.  Some observations on cortical inputs to the macaque monkey amygdala: An anterograde tracing study , 2002, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[53]  Cory T. Miller,et al.  The communicative content of the common marmoset phee call during antiphonal calling , 2010, American journal of primatology.

[54]  D H Whalen,et al.  Exploring the Relationship of Inspiration Duration to Utterance Duration , 1997, Phonetica.

[55]  Cory T. Miller,et al.  Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience Integrative Neuroscience , 2022 .

[56]  U. Jürgens Neural pathways underlying vocal control , 2002, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[57]  P. Aitken Cortical control of conditioned and spontaneous vocal behavior in rhesus monkeys , 1981, Brain and Language.

[58]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Auditory belt and parabelt projections to the prefrontal cortex in the Rhesus monkey , 1999, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[59]  Cory T. Miller,et al.  Antiphonal call timing in marmosets is behaviorally significant: interactive playback experiments , 2009, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[60]  A. Ghazanfar,et al.  Coupled Oscillator Dynamics of Vocal Turn-Taking in Monkeys , 2013, Current Biology.

[61]  Drew Rendall,et al.  Two organizing principles of vocal production: Implications for nonhuman and human primates , 2011, American journal of primatology.

[62]  D. Todt,et al.  Male–male vocal interactions and the adjustment of song amplitude in a territorial bird , 2004, Animal Behaviour.

[63]  L. Rosenblum,et al.  Relative Effectiveness of Three Stimulus Variables for Locating a Moving Sound Source , 1987, Perception.

[64]  A. Ghazanfar,et al.  Convergent Evolution of Vocal Cooperation without Convergent Evolution of Brain Size , 2014, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[65]  D. Rendall,et al.  Sound on the rebound: Bringing form and function back to the forefront in understanding nonhuman primate vocal signaling , 2001 .

[66]  Xiaoqin Wang,et al.  Neural substrates of vocalization feedback monitoring in primate auditory cortex , 2008, Nature.

[67]  D. Rendall,et al.  Out of Our Minds , 2010 .

[68]  A M Galaburda,et al.  The intrinsic architectonic and connectional organization of the superior temporal region of the rhesus monkey , 1983, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[69]  U. Jürgens,et al.  The effects of lesions in the cortical face area, supplementary motor cortex and anterior cingulate cortex on vocalization in the monkey , 1982, Behavioural Brain Research.

[70]  S. Ribeiro,et al.  Activation of Frontal Neocortical Areas by Vocal Production in Marmosets , 2010, Front. Integr. Neurosci..