Perceived susceptibility and self-protective behavior: a field experiment to encourage home radon testing.

Tested in a field experiment (N = 647) the hypothesis that perceptions of personal susceptibility are important in decisions to test one's home for radioactive radon gas. Experimental group subjects received a personal telephone call to tell them they lived in a high-risk area and a personal letter to reinforce the telephone message. After the intervention, experimental subjects were significantly more likely than minimal-treatment subjects to acknowledge the possibility of high radon levels in their homes. Perceptions of susceptibility and illness severity were significantly correlated with orders of radon test kits and with testing intentions. Nevertheless, there were no differences between groups in test orders or intentions. Results are discussed in terms of the difficulty of getting people to acknowledge susceptibility and the factors other than risk perceptions that influence self-protective behavior.

[1]  J Miles,et al.  Health risks of radon and other internally deposited alpha-emitters: Compiled by Committee on the Biological Effects of Ionising Radiations. Pp. 602. National Academy Press, 1988. Paperback £45.00 , 1988 .

[2]  J. Kirscht The Health Belief Model and Predictions of Health Actions , 1988 .

[3]  T. Munakata,et al.  [Models of health-related behavior]. , 1983, [Kango gijutsu] : [Nursing technique].

[4]  Neil D. Weinstein,et al.  Reducing unrealistic optimism about illness susceptibility. , 1983 .

[5]  O. D. Duncan Path Analysis: Sociological Examples , 1966, American Journal of Sociology.

[6]  N. Weinstein,et al.  Determinants of Self‐Protective Behavior: Home Radon Testing , 1990 .

[7]  J. Sorenson,et al.  Reproductive plans of genetic counseling clients not eligible for prenatal diagnosis. , 1987, American journal of medical genetics.

[8]  A. Frankel,et al.  A conceptualization of threat communications and protective health behavior. , 1981, Social psychology quarterly.

[9]  D. S. Gochman Health behavior : emerging research perspectives , 1988 .

[10]  S. K. Wurtele,et al.  Relative contributions of protection motivation theory components in predicting exercise intentions and behavior. , 1987, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[11]  P M Sandman,et al.  Optimistic biases in public perceptions of the risk from radon. , 1988, American journal of public health.

[12]  R. W. Rogers,et al.  Beyond Fear Appeals: Negative and Positive Persuasive Appeals to Health and Self‐Esteem , 1988 .

[13]  J. Richard Eiser,et al.  Social psychology and behavioral medicine , 1982 .

[14]  Sandy K. Wurtele,et al.  Increasing Women's Calcium Intake: The Role of Health Beliefs, Intentions, and Health Value1 , 1988 .

[15]  R. Kessler,et al.  Perceived Risk of AIDS: Assessing the Behavioral and Psychosocial Consequences in a Cohort of Gay Men1 , 1987 .

[16]  D. Mechanic Handbook of health, health care, and the health professions , 1984, Psychological Medicine.

[17]  Kenneth H. Beck,et al.  The Effects of Health Threat Seriousness and Personal Efficacy upon Intentions and Behavior1 , 1981 .

[18]  T. Drabek Human system responses to disaster , 1986 .

[19]  N. Weinstein The precaution adoption process. , 1988, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[20]  Peter M. Sandman,et al.  Public Response to the Risk from Geological Radon , 1987 .

[21]  M. Becker,et al.  The Health Belief Model: A Decade Later , 1984, Health education quarterly.

[22]  B. Devellis,et al.  Participation of high‐risk subjects in colon cancer screening , 1989, Cancer.

[23]  N. Weinstein Optimistic biases about personal risks. , 1989, Science.