The CRITICAL DIFFERENCE in models of speech production: A response to Roelofs and Piai

In our original article (Mahon, Garcea, & Navarrete, 2012), we replicated Dalrymple-Alford’s (1972) observation of semantic facilitation at zero SOA when participants name the ink color (e.g., ‘red’) of semantically related non-color words (e.g., ‘fire’) compared to semantically unrelated non-color word distractors (e.g., ‘lawn’; see also Glaser & Glaser, 1989). We replicated this classic finding to draw attention to the implications of this semantic facilitation effect, and semantic facilitation effects generally, for a model of word selection in speech production. The implication is straightforward: The theory of lexical selection by competition predicts that the distractor ‘fire’ should interferemorewith saying ‘red’ than thedistractor ‘lawn’. This (and other) observation(s) of semantic facilitation are incompatible with the theory of lexical selection by competition. WEAVERþþ (Roelofs, 1992, 2003) is the most developed model that implements lexical selection by competition, and as itmakes both qualitative and quantitative predictions, its ability to simulate response time effects has been taken as a litmus test of the viability of the hypothesis of lexical selection by competition. As Roelofs and Piai (2013) write of the original (i.e., Roelofs, 2003) WEAVERþþ simulations, “.facilitation of 41 msec or more was obtained at preexposure SOAs and no effect at zero SOA” (p. 1768). In other words, the previously published simulations of WEAVERþþ did not resemble, in relevant ways, the empirical data. On that

[1]  Claudio Mulatti,et al.  Picture–word interference and the response–exclusion hypothesis , 2012, Cortex.

[2]  Frank Wijnen,et al.  Phonological Encoding and Monitoring in Normal and Pathological Speech , 2005 .

[3]  W. Glaser,et al.  Context effects in stroop-like word and picture processing. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[4]  R. Hartsuiker,et al.  The distractor frequency effect in picture-word interference: Evidence for response exclusion. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[5]  R. Hartsuiker,et al.  Lexical selection and verbal self-monitoring: Effects of lexicality, context, and time pressure in picture-word interference , 2012 .

[6]  Peter Indefrey,et al.  The Spatial and Temporal Signatures of Word Production Components: A Critical Update , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[7]  Ardi Roelofs,et al.  Set size and repetition matter: comment on Caramazza and Costa (2000) , 2001, Cognition.

[8]  A. Roelofs,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking , 1992, Cognition.

[9]  Bradford Z. Mahon,et al.  Lexical selection is not by competition: a reinterpretation of semantic interference and facilitation effects in the picture-word interference paradigm. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[10]  Herbert Schriefers,et al.  Distractor strength and selective attention in picture-naming performance , 2012, Memory & cognition.

[11]  J R Simon,et al.  Effects of S-R mapping and response modality on performance in a Stroop Task. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  Albert Costa,et al.  Set size and repetition in the picture–word interference paradigm: implications for models of naming , 2001, Cognition.

[13]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Semantic interference in a delayed naming task: evidence for the response exclusion hypothesis. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[14]  Herbert Schriefers,et al.  Semantic interference in immediate and delayed naming and reading: Attention and task decisions , 2011 .

[15]  K. Campbell,et al.  Effects of attention and semantic relation on event-related potentials in a picture-word naming task , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[16]  Bradford Z. Mahon,et al.  Factors Determining Semantic Facilitation and Interference in the Cyclic Naming Paradigm , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[17]  Gary S Dell,et al.  Saying the right word at the right time: Syntagmatic and paradigmatic interference in sentence production , 2008, Language and cognitive processes.

[18]  Albert Costa,et al.  A further look at semantic context effects in language production: The role of response congruency , 2006 .

[19]  Frank Oppermann,et al.  Is there semantic interference in delayed naming? , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[20]  Ardi Roelofs,et al.  Associative facilitation in the Stroop task: Comment on Mahon et al. (2012) , 2013, Cortex.

[21]  Bradford Z. Mahon,et al.  Picture–word interference and the Response-Exclusion Hypothesis: A response to Mulatti and Coltheart , 2012, Cortex.

[22]  Bradford Z. Mahon,et al.  A rose by any other name is still a rose: A reinterpretation of Hantsch and Mädebach , 2013, Language and cognitive processes.

[23]  Robert J Hartsuiker,et al.  The distractor frequency effect in a delayed picture-word interference task: further evidence for a late locus of distractor exclusion , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[24]  Ardi Roelofs,et al.  Event-related potentials and oscillatory brain responses associated with semantic and Stroop-like interference effects in overt naming , 2012, Brain Research.

[25]  R. Hartsuiker,et al.  A late locus of the distractor frequency effect in picture–word interference: Evidence from event-related potentials , 2013, Brain and Language.

[26]  G S Dell,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. , 1986, Psychological review.

[27]  J. Grainger,et al.  ERP Evidence for Ultra-Fast Semantic Processing in the Picture–Word Interference Paradigm , 2010, Front. Psychology.

[28]  Stephen J. Lupker,et al.  Input, Decision, and Response Factors in Picture-Word Interference , 1981 .

[29]  Ardi Roelofs,et al.  Spoken word planning, comprehending, and self-monitoring: Evaluation of WEAVER++ , 2005 .

[30]  E. C. Dalrymple-Alford Associative facilitation and interference in the Stroop color-word task , 1972 .

[31]  Herbert Schriefers,et al.  Selective attention and distractor frequency in naming performance: comment on Dhooge and Hartsuiker (2010). , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[32]  Gerrit Hirschfeld,et al.  Interference and facilitation in overt speech production investigated with event-related potentials , 2008, Neuroreport.

[33]  Albert Costa,et al.  The semantic interference effect in the picture-word interference paradigm: does the response set matter? , 2000, Cognition.

[34]  A. Roelofs,et al.  Goal-referenced selection of verbal action: modeling attentional control in the Stroop task. , 2003, Psychological review.