Head-Up-Displays Support Response Preparation in a Lane Change Task

In addition to visual design characteristics of head-up-displays (HUDs) in vehicles, the impact of HUDs on response preparation in driving has to be investigated. The current pilot study examined the effects of both temporal response preparation and movement planning on driving performance in a lane change task. Participants received a temporal warning stimulus within a HUD conveying full or no information about the direction of an upcoming lane change maneuver. After a variable foreperiod, they had to react as quickly as possible to the subsequent imperative stimulus, which was also shown in the HUD. The results indicated that temporal preparation was supported best at foreperiods of 600 ms and 1200 ms. Full advance information led to a reaction time advantage of approximately 70 ms. As temporal preparation and movement planning interacted, too, both mechanisms of response preparation potentially affect the same stage of the human information processing chain. Possible applications of the results are discussed.

[1]  P. J. Foley The foreperiod and simple reaction time. , 1959, Canadian journal of psychology.

[2]  A. Sanders 20 Stage Analysis of Reaction Processes , 1980 .

[3]  N J Ward,et al.  Head-up displays and their automotive application: an overview of human factors issues affecting safety. , 1994, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[4]  D. Rosenbaum Human movement initiation: specification of arm, direction, and extent. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[5]  Michael Sivak,et al.  Effects of driver-side mirror type on lane-change accidents , 1995 .

[6]  Rolf Ulrich,et al.  Locus of the effect of temporal preparation: evidence from the lateralized readiness potential. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[7]  W. Sommer,et al.  Partial advance information and response preparation: inferences from the lateralized readiness potential. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[8]  J Stahl,et al.  Preparation of response force and movement direction: onset effects on the lateralized readiness potential. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[9]  Otto Graf Eine Methode zur Untersuchung der pharmakologischen Beeinflussung von Koordinationsleistungen , 2007, Arbeitsphysiologie.

[10]  Steven W. Keele,et al.  Movement control in skilled motor performance. , 1968 .

[11]  Christopher D. Wickens,et al.  Object versus space-based models of visual attention: Implications for the design of head-up displays , 1995 .

[12]  Alexa Riehle,et al.  Preparation for Action: one of the Key Functions of Motor Cortex , 2004 .

[13]  David Crundall,et al.  Selective searching while driving: the role of experience in hazard detection and general surveillance , 2002, Ergonomics.

[14]  K W Gish,et al.  Human Factors Aspects Of Using Head Up Displays In Automobiles: A Review Of The Literature, Interim Report , 1995 .

[15]  B. Rolke,et al.  Temporal uncertainty degrades perceptual processing , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[16]  Erwin R. Boer,et al.  Toward an Integrated Model of Driver Behavior in Cognitive Architecture , 2001 .

[17]  W.A.C. Spijkers,et al.  Specification of direction and duration during programming of discrete sliding movements , 1984 .

[18]  Daniel R. Tufano,et al.  Automotive HUDs: The Overlooked Safety Issues , 1997, Hum. Factors.

[19]  J Godthelp Precognitive control: open- and closed-loop steering in a lane-change manoeuvre. , 1985, Ergonomics.

[20]  Rolf Ulrich,et al.  Time resolution of clocks: Effects on reaction time measurement—Good news for bad clocks , 1989 .