Neurocognitive mechanisms for processing inflectional and derivational complexity in English

In the current paper we discuss the mechanisms that underlie the processing of inflectional and derivational complexity in English. We address this issue from a neurocognitive perspective and present evidence from a new fMRI study that the two types of morphological complexity engage the language processing network in different ways. The processing of inflectional complexity selectively activates a left-lateralised frontotemporal system, specialised for combinatorial grammatical computations, while derivational complexity primarily engages a distributed bilateral system, argued to support whole-word, stem based lexical access. We discuss the implications of our findings for theories of the processing and representation of morphologically complex words.

[1]  William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Morphological Processes in language Comprehension , 2007 .

[2]  Volkmar Glauche,et al.  Ventral and dorsal pathways for language , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[3]  D. T. Ives,et al.  Bihemispheric foundations for human speech comprehension , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[4]  Lars Borin,et al.  What is a lexical representation? , 1985, NODALIDA.

[5]  M. Taft Morphological Decomposition and the Reverse Base Frequency Effect , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[6]  Geert Booij,et al.  Inflection and Derivation , 2000 .

[7]  S. Scott,et al.  The neuroanatomical and functional organization of speech perception , 2003, Trends in Neurosciences.

[8]  R. Baayen,et al.  Singulars and plurals in Dutch: Evidence for a parallel dual-route model , 1997 .

[9]  Roy D. Patterson,et al.  Locating the initial stages of speech–sound processing in human temporal cortex , 2006, NeuroImage.

[10]  Emmanuel A Stamatakis,et al.  Functional organization of the neural language system: dorsal and ventral pathways are critical for syntax. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[11]  William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Neurocognitive Contexts for Morphological Complexity: Dissociating Inflection and Derivation , 2010, Lang. Linguistics Compass.

[12]  J. Segui,et al.  On the representation and processing of prefixed and suffixed derived words: A differential frequency effect , 1989 .

[13]  W. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Morphology and meaning in the English mental lexicon. , 1994 .

[14]  Li Su,et al.  Neurobiological Systems for Lexical Representation and Analysis in English , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  Angela D. Friederici,et al.  Pathways to language: fiber tracts in the human brain , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  R. Baayen,et al.  The balance of storage and computation in morphological processing: the role of word formation type, affixal homonymy, and productivity. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[17]  Dissociation of inflectional and derivational morphology in English: Evidence from a single-case study , 2007, Brain and Language.

[18]  Lorraine K. Tyler,et al.  Processing bound grammatical morphemes in context: The case of an aphasic patient , 1987 .

[19]  Alan Connelly,et al.  A Direct Test for Lateralization of Language Activation using fMRI: Comparison with Invasive Assessments in Children with Epilepsy , 2002, NeuroImage.

[20]  Harald Clahsen,et al.  Derivational morphology in the German mental lexicon: A dual mechanism account , 2002 .

[21]  Matthew H. Davis,et al.  Morphological and semantic effects in visual word recognition: A time-course study , 2000 .

[22]  Lorraine K Tyler,et al.  Morphology, language and the brain: the decompositional substrate for language comprehension , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[23]  Derek K. Jones,et al.  Perisylvian language networks of the human brain , 2005, Annals of neurology.

[24]  Matthias Schlesewsky,et al.  Processing linguistic complexity and grammaticality in the left frontal cortex. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[25]  L. Tyler,et al.  Differentiating Hemispheric Contributions to Syntax and Semantics in Patients with Left-Hemisphere Lesions , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Early decomposition in visual word recognition: Dissociating morphology, form, and meaning , 2008, Language and cognitive processes.

[27]  H. Goodglass,et al.  Comparison of Morphology and Syntax in Free Narrative and Structured Tests: Fluent vs. Nonfluent Aphasics , 1993, Cortex.

[28]  D. Poeppel,et al.  The cortical organization of speech processing , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[29]  Lorraine K. Tyler,et al.  Dynamic Processing in the Human Language System: Synergy between the Arcuate Fascicle and Extreme Capsule , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Representation and processing of derived words , 1987 .

[31]  Stephen M. Rao,et al.  Human Brain Language Areas Identified by Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  M. Taft Recognition of affixed words and the word frequency effect , 1979, Memory & cognition.

[33]  B. Butterworth Development, writing, and other language processes , 1983 .

[34]  S. Mattys,et al.  Lexical activity in speech processing: Evidence from pause detection , 2002 .

[35]  F. Meunier,et al.  Morphological decomposition in early visual word processing , 2005 .

[36]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Determinants of dominance: Is language laterality explained by physical or linguistic features of speech? , 2005, NeuroImage.

[37]  William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Temporal and frontal systems in speech comprehension: An fMRI study of past tense processing , 2005, Neuropsychologia.