Saccade target selection in macaque during feature and conjunction visual search

To gain insight into how vision guides eye movements, monkeys were trained to make a single saccade to a specified target stimulus during feature and conjunction search with stimuli discriminated by color and shape. Monkeys performed both tasks at levels well above chance. The latencies of saccades to the target in conjunction search exhibited shallow positive slopes as a function of set size, comparable to slopes of reaction time of humans during target present/absent judgments, but significantly different than the slopes in feature search. Properties of the selection process were revealed by the occasional saccades to distractors. During feature search, errant saccades were directed more often to a distractor near the target than to a distractor at any other location. In contrast, during conjunction search, saccades to distractors were guided more by similarity than proximity to the target; monkeys were significantly more likely to shift gaze to a distractor that had one of the target features than to a distractor that had none. Overall, color and shape information were used to similar degrees in the search for the conjunction target. However, in single sessions we observed an increased tendency of saccades to a distractor that had been the target in the previous experimental session. The establishment of this tendency across sessions at least a day apart and its persistence throughout a session distinguish this phenomenon from the short-term (<10 trials) perceptual priming observed in this and earlier studies using feature visual search. Our findings support the hypothesis that the target in at least some conjunction visual searches can be detected efficiently based on visual similarity, most likely through parallel processing of the individual features that define the stimuli. These observations guide the interpretation of neurophysiological data and constrain the development of computational models.

[1]  D. Robinson,et al.  A METHOD OF MEASURING EYE MOVEMENT USING A SCLERAL SEARCH COIL IN A MAGNETIC FIELD. , 1963, IEEE transactions on bio-medical engineering.

[2]  L G Williams,et al.  The effects of target specification on objects fixated during visual search. , 1967, Acta psychologica.

[3]  J. Townsend Serial and within-stage independent parallel model equivalence on the minimum completion time ☆ , 1976 .

[4]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .

[5]  W. W. Daniel Applied Nonparametric Statistics , 1979 .

[6]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[7]  D. Robinson,et al.  Behavioral enhancement of visual responses in monkey cerebral cortex. I. Modulation in posterior parietal cortex related to selective visual attention. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  A. Treisman Perceptual grouping and attention in visual search for features and for objects. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  H. Egeth,et al.  Searching for conjunctively defined targets. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  Ken Nakayama,et al.  Serial and parallel processing of visual feature conjunctions , 1986, Nature.

[11]  B. C. Motter,et al.  Common and differential effects of attentive fixation on the excitability of parietal and prestriate (V4) cortical visual neurons in the macaque monkey , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[12]  Jon Driver,et al.  Visual search for a conjunction of movement and form is parallel , 1988, Nature.

[13]  J. Duncan,et al.  Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.

[14]  Susan L. Franzel,et al.  Guided search: an alternative to the feature integration model for visual search. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  M. Goldberg,et al.  The visual and frontal cortices. , 1989, Reviews of oculomotor research.

[16]  J. Townsend Serial vs. Parallel Processing: Sometimes They Look like Tweedledum and Tweedledee but they can (and Should) be Distinguished , 1990 .

[17]  A. Treisman,et al.  Conjunction search revisited. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  Jeremy M Wolfe,et al.  Modeling the role of parallel processing in visual search , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.

[19]  S. Shott,et al.  Nonparametric Statistics , 2018, The Encyclopedia of Archaeological Sciences.

[20]  A. Treisman Search, similarity, and integration of features between and within dimensions. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Organization of visual inputs to the inferior temporal and posterior parietal cortex in macaques , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[22]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Coding visual images of objects in the inferotemporal cortex of the macaque monkey. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  J. Theeuwes Cross-dimensional perceptual selectivity , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[24]  M. Bravo,et al.  The role of attention in different visual-search tasks , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[25]  A. Treisman Spreading suppression or feature integration? A reply to Duncan and Humphreys (1992). , 1992 .

[26]  J. Duncan,et al.  Beyond the search surface: visual search and attentional engagement. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  D. Robinson,et al.  Covert orienting of attention in macaques. I. Effects of behavioral context. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  John Duncan,et al.  A neural basis for visual search in inferior temporal cortex , 1993, Nature.

[29]  K. Pribram,et al.  Cortical involvement in visual scan in the monkey , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[30]  H. Komatsu,et al.  Relationships between color, shape, and pattern selectivities of neurons in the inferior temporal cortex of the monkey. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features , 1994, Memory & cognition.

[32]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[33]  B. Motter Neural correlates of attentive selection for color or luminance in extrastriate area V4 , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[34]  J. Schall,et al.  Saccade target selection in frontal eye field of macaque. I. Visual and premovement activation , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[35]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[36]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Superior Parietal Cortex Activation During Spatial Attention Shifts and Visual Feature Conjunction , 1995, Science.

[37]  J. Bullier,et al.  Topography of visual cortex connections with frontal eye field in macaque: convergence and segregation of processing streams , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[38]  J. Schall,et al.  Countermanding saccades in macaque , 1995, Visual Neuroscience.

[39]  J H Maunsell,et al.  The Brain's Visual World: Representation of Visual Targets in Cerebral Cortex , 1995, Science.

[40]  K. Cave,et al.  Spatial Attention in Visual Search for Features and Feature Conjunctions , 1995 .

[41]  S. Luck,et al.  The role of attention in feature detection and conjunction discrimination: an electrophysiological analysis. , 1995, The International journal of neuroscience.

[42]  B. Dosher,et al.  The role of attention in the programming of saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[43]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Priming of pop-out: II. The role of position , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[44]  D. Robinson,et al.  Shared neural control of attentional shifts and eye movements , 1996, Nature.

[45]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Attentional modulation of visual motion processing in cortical areas MT and MST , 1996, Nature.

[46]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Priming of popout: II. Role of position , 1996 .

[47]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Visual feature selectivity in frontal eye fields induced by experience in mature macaques , 1996, Nature.

[48]  H. Deubel,et al.  Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.

[49]  G. Zelinsky Using Eye Saccades to Assess the Selectivity of Search Movements , 1996, Vision Research.

[50]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Perceptual and motor processing stages identified in the activity of macaque frontal eye field neurons during visual search. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[51]  J. Schall Visuomotor Areas of the Frontal Lobe , 1997 .

[52]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Dissociation of visual discrimination from saccade programming in macaque frontal eye field. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[53]  J. Findlay Saccade Target Selection During Visual Search , 1997, Vision Research.

[54]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[55]  J. Kaas,et al.  Extrastriate Cortex in Primates , 1997, Cerebral Cortex.

[56]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Spatial selection via feature-driven inhibition of distractor locations , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.

[57]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Neural correlates of visual and motor decision processes , 1998, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[58]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Visual selection mediated by location: Feature-based selection of noncontiguous locations , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.