Direction‐specific motion blindness induced by focal stimulation of human extrastriate cortex

Motion blindness (MB) or akinetopsia is the selective disturbance of visual motion perception while other features of the visual scene such as colour and shape are normally perceived. Chronic and transient forms of MB are characterized by a global deficit of direction discrimination (pandirectional), which is generally assumed to result from damage to, or interference with, the motion complex MT+/V5. However, the most characteristic feature of primate MT‐neurons is not their motion specificity, but their preference for one direction of motion (direction specificity). Here, we report that focal electrical stimulation in the human posterior temporal lobe selectively impaired the perception of motion in one direction while the perception of motion in other directions was completely normal (unidirectional MB). In addition, the direction of MB was found to depend on the brain area stimulated. It is argued that direction specificity for visual motion is not only represented at the single neuron level, but also in much larger cortical units.

[1]  S. Zeki,et al.  Response properties and receptive fields of cells in an anatomically defined region of the superior temporal sulcus in the monkey. , 1971, Brain research.

[2]  S. Zeki Functional organization of a visual area in the posterior bank of the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesus monkey , 1974, The Journal of physiology.

[3]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  The middle temporal visual area in the macaque: Myeloarchitecture, connections, functional properties and topographic organization , 1981, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[4]  N. Mai,et al.  Selective disturbance of movement vision after bilateral brain damage. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[5]  R. Desimone,et al.  Columnar organization of directionally selective cells in visual area MT of the macaque. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  G. Klem,et al.  Extraoperative Cortical Functional Localization in Patients with Epilepsy , 1987, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[7]  W. Newsome,et al.  A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT) , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[8]  William T. Newsome,et al.  Cortical microstimulation influences perceptual judgements of motion direction , 1990, Nature.

[9]  S. Zeki,et al.  Cerebral akinetopsia (visual motion blindness). A review. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[10]  W. Newsome,et al.  Microstimulation in visual area MT: effects on direction discrimination performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[11]  V. Hömberg,et al.  Cerebral visual motion blindness: transitory akinetopsia induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation of human area V5 , 1992, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[12]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Impaired visual motion perception in the contralateral hemifield following unilateral posterior cerebral lesions in humans. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[13]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Area V5 of the human brain: evidence from a combined study using positron emission tomography and magnetic resonance imaging. , 1993, Cerebral cortex.

[14]  A Grinvald,et al.  Optical imaging reveals the functional architecture of neurons processing shape and motion in owl monkey area MT , 1994, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[15]  W. Merigan,et al.  Motion perception following lesions of the superior temporal sulcus in the monkey. , 1994, Cerebral cortex.

[16]  D. Braun,et al.  Transcranial magnetic stimulation of extrastriate cortex degrades human motion direction discrimination , 1994, Vision Research.

[17]  J. Sharpe,et al.  Retinotopic and directional defects in motion discrimination in humans with cerebral lesions , 1995, Annals of neurology.

[18]  R. Andersen,et al.  Functional analysis of human MT and related visual cortical areas using magnetic resonance imaging , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[19]  T. Schenk,et al.  Visual motion perception after brain damage: I. Deficits in global motion perception , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  A. T. Smith,et al.  Detection and Discrimination of First- and Second-Order Motion in Patients with Unilateral Brain Damage , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  T. Allison,et al.  Temporal Cortex Activation in Humans Viewing Eye and Mouth Movements , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  T. Pasternak,et al.  Transient and permanent deficits in motion perception after lesions of cortical areas MT and MST in the macaque monkey. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[23]  J. Zihl,et al.  Cerebral Akinetopsia (Visual motion blindness). , 1999 .

[24]  O. Blanke,et al.  Location of the human frontal eye field as defined by electrical cortical stimulation: anatomical, functional and electrophysiological characteristics , 2000, Neuroreport.

[25]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  A new anatomical landmark for reliable identification of human area V5/MT: a quantitative analysis of sulcal patterning. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[26]  A. Cowey,et al.  Regional cerebral correlates of global motion perception: evidence from unilateral cerebral brain damage. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[27]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Fast Backprojections from the Motion to the Primary Visual Area Necessary for Visual Awareness , 2001, Science.

[28]  J. Haxby,et al.  Parallel Visual Motion Processing Streams for Manipulable Objects and Human Movements , 2002, Neuron.

[29]  L. Vaina Selective impairment of visual motion interpretation following lesions of the right occipito-parietal area in humans , 1989, Biological Cybernetics.