Associations Among Gender-Linked Toy Preferences, Spatial Ability, and Digit Ratio: Evidence from Eye-Tracking Analysis

Eye-tracking technology was used to monitor eye-movements in 64 adults (age range, 18–22 years) during simultaneous presentation of “masculine” and “feminine” toys. Women and men who showed more visual fixations on male-typical toys compared to female-typical toys had significantly better targeting ability and smaller (i.e., more masculine) digit ratios, a putative marker of prenatal androgen levels. In contrast, individuals with visual preferences for female-typical or male-typical toys did not differ in mental rotations ability and in their retrospective reports of childhood gender-linked activities. The finding that targeting ability and digit ratios varied according to visual preferences for gender-linked toys suggests that prenatal androgens promote enduring preferences for male-typical objects and indicate further that some gender-linked traits vary according to the direction of a visual preference for gender-linked toys. Visual preferences derived from eye-tracking, therefore, may be a useful supplement to current measures of psychosexual differentiation in hormone-behavior research, particularly because eye-movements are not dependent on verbal abilities or subjective evaluations of behavior.

[1]  Anke A. Ehrhardt,et al.  Fetal androgens, human central nervous system differentiation, and behavior sex differences. , 1974 .

[2]  Daniel Voyer,et al.  The Relation Between Experience and Spatial Performance in Men and Women , 2000 .

[3]  L. Serbin,et al.  Sex-Typing of Children's Play Preferences and Patterns of Cognitive Performance , 1979 .

[4]  E. Hampson,et al.  Testing the prenatal androgen hypothesis: measuring digit ratios, sexual orientation, and spatial abilities in adults , 2005, Hormones and Behavior.

[5]  N. Watson Sex differences in throwing: monkeys having a fling , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  J. Loehlin,et al.  A Reanalysis of Five Studies on Sexual Orientation and the Relative Length of the 2nd and 4th Fingers (the 2D:4D Ratio) , 2005, Archives of sexual behavior.

[7]  R. Dittmann,et al.  Congenital adrenal hyperplasia II: Gender-related behavior and attitudes in female salt-wasting and simple-virilizing patients , 1990, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[8]  B. Morgan A Three Generational Study of Tomboy Behavior , 1998 .

[9]  Darren J. Newton,et al.  The second to fourth digit ratio and variation in the androgen receptor gene , 2003 .

[10]  C. L. Martin,et al.  The relation of gender understanding to children's sex-typed preferences and gender stereotypes. , 1990, Child development.

[11]  M. O'brien,et al.  Activity level and sex-stereotyped toy choice in toddler boys and girls. , 1985, The Journal of genetic psychology.

[12]  P. Hindmarsh,et al.  Prenatal hormones and postnatal socialization by parents as determinants of male-typical toy play in girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. , 2005, Child development.

[13]  G. C. Quarton,et al.  The Neurosciences;: Second study program , 1970 .

[14]  S. Vandenberg,et al.  Mental Rotations, a Group Test of Three-Dimensional Spatial Visualization , 1978, Perceptual and motor skills.

[15]  Patricia S. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Viewing preferences of rhesus monkeys related to memory for complex pictures, colours and faces , 1994, Behavioural Brain Research.

[16]  Dyanne M. Tracy,et al.  Toys, spatial ability, and science and mathematics achievement: Are they related? , 1987 .

[17]  Michael Peters,et al.  Sexual dimorphism in the 2D/4D ratio and its relation to mental rotation performance , 2003 .

[18]  J. R. Thomas,et al.  Gender differences across age in motor performance a meta-analysis. , 1985, Psychological bulletin.

[19]  N. Mackworth,et al.  Cognitive determinants of fixation location during picture viewing. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  M. Hines,et al.  Human behavioral sex differences: a role for gonadal hormones during early development? , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[21]  J. M. Connor,et al.  Behaviorally Based Masculine- and Feminine-Activity-Preference Scales for Preschoolers: Correlates with Other Classroom Behaviors and Cognitive Tests. , 1977 .

[22]  N. Eisenberg,et al.  Children's Reasoning regarding Sex-typed Toy Choices , 1982 .

[23]  S. Gaulin,et al.  Sex hormones and finger length: What does 2D:4D indicate? , 2004 .

[24]  J. Rainer Sex Differences in Behavior , 1975 .

[25]  J. Rust,et al.  Testosterone during pregnancy and gender role behavior of preschool children: a longitudinal, population study. , 2002, Child development.

[26]  B. Fagot The influence of sex of child on parental reactions to toddler children. , 1978 .

[27]  János Kállai,et al.  Spatial navigation related to the ratio of second to fourth digit length in women , 2003 .

[28]  A. Pomerleau,et al.  Pink or blue: Environmental gender stereotypes in the first two years of life , 1990 .

[29]  J. Langlois,et al.  Mothers, fathers, and peers as socialization agents of sex-typed play behaviors in young children. , 1980 .

[30]  Daniel Voyer,et al.  Gender differences on the mental rotations test: a factor analysis. , 2004, Acta psychologica.

[31]  H. Markovits,et al.  Sex differences in infants’ attraction to group versus individual stimuli , 2004 .

[32]  Diane Poulin-Dubois,et al.  Gender stereotyping in infancy: Visual preferences for and knowledge of gender-stereotyped toys in the second year , 2001 .

[33]  Marsha B. Liss Social and cognitive skills : sex roles and children's play , 1983 .

[34]  M. O'brien,et al.  Development of sex-typed play behavior in toddlers. , 1985 .

[35]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.

[36]  K. Kerns,et al.  Sex differences in spatial ability in children , 1991, Behavior genetics.

[37]  E. Maccoby,et al.  The Psychology of Sex Differences , 1974 .

[38]  S. Gaulin,et al.  Sexual selection for spatial-learning ability , 1989, Animal Behaviour.

[39]  Visuo‐spatial play experience: forerunner of visuo‐spatial achievement in preadolescent and adolescent boys and girls? , 2004 .

[40]  R. Mansfield,et al.  Analysis of visual behavior , 1982 .

[41]  E. Maccoby,et al.  Gender as a social category. , 1988 .

[42]  Daniel C. Richardson,et al.  Representation, space and Hollywood Squares: looking at things that aren't there anymore , 2000, Cognition.

[43]  C. Acerini,et al.  In-utero androgen exposure and 2nd to 4th digit length ratio-comparisons between healthy controls and females with classical congenital adrenal hyperplasia. , 2003, Human reproduction.

[44]  A. Okten,et al.  The ratio of second- and fourth-digit lengths and congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21-hydroxylase deficiency. , 2002, Early human development.

[45]  J. Sherman,et al.  Problem of sex differences in space perception and aspects of intellectual functioning. , 1967, Psychological review.

[46]  Gina M. Grimshaw,et al.  Mental Rotation at 7 Years - Relations with Prenatal Testosterone Levels and Spatial Play Experiences , 1995, Brain and Cognition.

[47]  E. Maccoby,et al.  The Two Sexes: Growing Up Apart, Coming Together. Family and Public Policy Series. , 1998 .

[48]  S. M. Breedlove,et al.  Masculinized Finger Length Patterns in Human Males and Females with Congenital Adrenal Hyperplasia , 2002, Hormones and Behavior.

[49]  J. Benenson,et al.  Propulsion : A behavioural expression of masculinity , 1997 .

[50]  S. Berenbaum,et al.  Prenatal sex hormone effects on child and adult sex-typed behavior: methods and findings , 2005, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[51]  N. Newcombe,et al.  The role of experience in spatial test performance: A meta-analysis , 1989 .

[52]  N. Kutner,et al.  Current Conceptions of Sex Roles and Sex Typing: Theory and Research , 1987 .

[53]  J. Money,et al.  Progestin‐induced hermaphroditism: IQ and psychosexual identity in a study of ten girls∗∗ , 1967 .

[54]  M. Linn,et al.  Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: a meta-analysis. , 1985, Child development.

[55]  K. Zucker Measurement of Psychosexual Differentiation , 2005, Archives of sexual behavior.

[56]  M. New,et al.  Prenatal Androgenization Affects Gender-Related Behavior But Not Gender Identity in 5–12-Year-Old Girls with Congenital Adrenal Hyperplasia , 2004, Archives of sexual behavior.

[57]  B. Fagot,et al.  Relation of Preschool Sex-Typing to Intellectual Performance in Elementary School , 1976 .

[58]  Jaak Panksepp,et al.  The psychobiology of play: Theoretical and methodological perspectives , 1984, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[59]  G. Alexander An Evolutionary Perspective of Sex-Typed Toy Preferences: Pink, Blue, and the Brain , 2003, Archives of sexual behavior.

[60]  M. Peters,et al.  A Redrawn Vandenberg and Kuse Mental Rotations Test - Different Versions and Factors That Affect Performance , 1995, Brain and Cognition.

[61]  D. Kimura,et al.  Sexual orientation and performance on sexually dimorphic motor tasks , 1995, Archives of sexual behavior.

[62]  A Bandura,et al.  Social cognitive theory of gender development and differentiation. , 1999, Psychological review.

[63]  M. Hines,et al.  Spatial abilities following prenatal androgen abnormality: targeting and mental rotations performance in individuals with congenital adrenal hyperplasia , 2003, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[64]  M. Hines,et al.  Androgen and the development of human sex-typical behavior: rough-and-tumble play and sex of preferred playmates in children with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH). , 1994, Child development.

[65]  Sheri A. Berenbaum,et al.  Early Androgens Are Related to Childhood Sex-Typed Toy Preferences , 1992 .

[66]  M. Hines,et al.  Androgen and psychosexual development: Core gender identity, sexual orientation, and recalled childhood gender role behavior in women and men with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH) , 2004, Journal of sex research.

[67]  Beverly I. Fagot,et al.  Gender labeling, gender stereotyping, and parenting behaviors. , 1992 .

[68]  J. Rust,et al.  The Pre-School Activities Inventory: A standardized assessment of gender role in children. , 1993 .

[69]  M. Hines,et al.  Gender labels and play styles: their relative contribution to children's selection of playmates. , 1994, Child development.

[70]  Yvonne M. Caldera,et al.  Children’s Play Preferences, Construction Play with Blocks, and Visual-spatial Skills: Are they Related? , 1999 .

[71]  G. Alexander,et al.  Sex differences in response to children's toys in nonhuman primates (Cercopithecus aethiops sabaeus) , 2002 .

[72]  C. Jordan,et al.  The Orthodox View of Brain Sexual Differentiation , 1999, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[73]  J T Manning,et al.  The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length: a predictor of sperm numbers and concentrations of testosterone, luteinizing hormone and oestrogen. , 1998, Human reproduction.

[74]  C. Crook,et al.  Infants' visual preference for sex-congruent babies, children, toys and activities: A longitudinal study. , 2000 .

[75]  Susan D. Voyer,et al.  Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: a meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[76]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Sex differences in human neonatal social perception , 2000 .

[77]  Marsha B. Liss Patterns of toy play: An analysis of sex differences , 1981 .