The relative timing of active and passive touch

Tactile stimulation usually occurs as a combination of an active movement (reaching out to touch a surface) and a sensation (actually feeling the surface against the skin). The brain has information about the active component (the motor command) before it occurs because of efference copy, while the passive component must be transduced before it can be processed. Since the active and passive tactile components are available to the brain at different times, determining the time of touch requires calculation worked backwards from the passive sensation, and/or worked forward from the active motor command. In order to determine which touch process is perceived more quickly, we varied the relative delay between an active and a passive touch signal and determined the relative time perceived as simultaneous. A passive touch needed to be presented before an active key was pressed in order for the two touches to be perceived as simultaneous, but this timing difference was not significant. In order to test the plasticity of the active and passive touch systems, we exploited the fact that the point of subjective simultaneity between two stimuli can sometimes be altered by repeated exposure to asynchronous presentation. We exposed subjects to an active key press/ passive touch pair delayed by 250 ms. This exposure increased the range of relative delays between active and passive touches at which the pairs were judged as simultaneous. This is consistent with an adaptive change in the processing of active touch.

[1]  D. Cunningham,et al.  Sensorimotor Adaptation to Violations of Temporal Contiguity , 2001, Psychological science.

[2]  L. Harris,et al.  Simultaneity Constancy , 2004, Perception.

[3]  E. Holst,et al.  Das Reafferenzprinzip , 2004, Naturwissenschaften.

[4]  J. Vroomen,et al.  Temporal recalibration to tactile–visual asynchronous stimuli , 2008, Neuroscience Letters.

[5]  R. Passingham,et al.  Attention to Intention , 2004, Science.

[6]  J R Duhamel,et al.  The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. , 1992, Science.

[7]  H. Kornhuber,et al.  Hirnpotentialänderungen bei Willkürbewegungen und passiven Bewegungen des Menschen: Bereitschaftspotential und reafferente Potentiale , 1965, Pflüger's Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie des Menschen und der Tiere.

[8]  P. Montague,et al.  Motor-Sensory Recalibration Leads to an Illusory Reversal of Action and Sensation , 2006, Neuron.

[9]  C. E. Chapman,et al.  Time course and magnitude of movement-related gating of tactile detection in humans. I. Importance of stimulus location. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  P. Bertelson,et al.  Recalibration of temporal order perception by exposure to audio-visual asynchrony. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[11]  M Di Luca,et al.  Recalibration of audiovisual simultaneity , 2010 .

[12]  M Concetta Morrone,et al.  Saccadic eye movements cause compression of time as well as space , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  B. Libet,et al.  Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[14]  Patrick Haggard,et al.  Supplementary motor area provides an efferent signal for sensory suppression. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[15]  L. Harris,et al.  The effect of exposure to asynchronous audio, visual, and tactile stimulus combinations on the perception of simultaneity , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  L. Harris,et al.  Simultaneity constancy: detecting events with touch and vision , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  D. Burke,et al.  Conduction velocities of muscle and cutaneous afferents in the upper and lower limbs of human subjects. , 1989, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[18]  B. Libet,et al.  Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. , 1983 .

[19]  H. Kornhuber,et al.  [CHANGES IN THE BRAIN POTENTIAL IN VOLUNTARY MOVEMENTS AND PASSIVE MOVEMENTS IN MAN: READINESS POTENTIAL AND REAFFERENT POTENTIALS]. , 1965, Pflugers Archiv fur die gesamte Physiologie des Menschen und der Tiere.

[20]  Charles Spence,et al.  Adaptation to audiotactile asynchrony , 2007, Neuroscience Letters.