Detour behaviour, imprinting and visual lateralization in the domestic chick.

Detour behaviour was studied in chicks faced with a vertical-bar barrier behind where an imprinting object (a red ball) was located. Right-eyed chicks took less time to detour the barrier than left-eyed chicks, and binocular chicks showed a bias to detour the barrier on the left side, thus maintaining visual contact with the imprinting object using the lateral field of the right eye, while circling around the barrier. In males, the asymmetries were consistent all along the first two weeks of life, whereas in females they disappeared on days 8 and 11. When tested with a slightly novel version of the original imprinting object (i.e., a ball of a different color), binocular chicks showed a bias to detour the barrier on the right side, thus showing preferential use of the left eye. The same bias occurred when unfamiliar conspecifics were used as goal-objects. Results suggest that cerebral lateralization in birds can directly affect visually-guided motor responses through selective use of the lateral field of vision of the eye contralateral to the hemisphere which has to be put in charge of control of overt behaviour.

[1]  G. Vallortigara,et al.  Olfactory lateralization in the chick , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  L. Rogers,et al.  Asymmetry in the chicken forebrain during development and a possible involvement of the supraoptic decussation , 1983, Neuroscience Letters.

[3]  G. Vallortigara,et al.  Laterality in detour behaviour: interspecific variation in poeciliid fish , 1997, Animal Behaviour.

[4]  Joseph B. Hellige,et al.  Right hemisphere superiority for initial stages of letter processing , 1979, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  P. Bishop,et al.  The optic nerve. Properties of a central tract * , 1953, The Journal of physiology.

[6]  G. Horn,et al.  Neurophysiological Investigations of a Recognition Memory System for Imprinting in the Domestic Chick , 1995, The European journal of neuroscience.

[7]  G. Vallortigara,et al.  Lateralized agonistic responses and hindlimb use in toads , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[8]  Onur Güntürkün,et al.  Visual memory lateralization in pigeons , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  Lateral asymmetries during responses to novel-coloured objects in the domestic chick: A developmental study , 1996, Behavioural Processes.

[10]  L. Regolin,et al.  Sharply Timed Behavioral Changes During the First 5 Weeks of Life in the Domestic Chick (Gallus gallus) , 1997 .

[11]  G. Vallortigara,et al.  Detour tests reveal task- and stimulus-specific behavioural lateralization in mosquitofish (Gambusia holbrooki) , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.

[12]  G. Vallortigara Affiliation and aggression as related to gender in domestic chicks (Gallus gallus). , 1992, Journal of comparative psychology.

[13]  Ruth Ellen Proudfoot Hemiretinal differences in face recognition: Accuracy versus reaction time , 1983, Brain and Cognition.

[14]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Asymmetries in the visual processing of emotional cues during agonistic interactions by gelada baboons , 1996, Behavioural Processes.

[15]  J. Bradshaw,et al.  The Evolution of Lateral Asymmetries, Language, Tool Use, and Intellect. , 1994 .

[16]  R. Andrew,et al.  The development of visual lateralization in the domestic chick , 1988, Behavioural Brain Research.

[17]  L. Rogers,et al.  Glutamate-induced asymmetry in the sexual and aggressive behavior of young chickens , 1986, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[18]  R. Andrew,et al.  The role of stimulus size and colour in the elicitation of testosterone-facilitated aggressive and sexual responses in the domestic chick , 1983, Animal Behaviour.

[19]  Johan J. Bolhuis,et al.  MECHANISMS OF AVIAN IMPRINTING: A REVIEW , 1991, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[20]  G. Vallortigara,et al.  The Origins of Cerebral Asymmetry: A Review of Evidence of Behavioural and Brain Lateralization in Fishes, Reptiles and Amphibians , 1998, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[21]  L. Regolin,et al.  Perception of partly occluded objects by young chicks , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[22]  G. Vallortigara,et al.  Differential involvement of right and left hemisphere in individual recognition in the domestic chick , 1994, Behavioural Processes.

[23]  I. Lewin,et al.  Effects of voluntary eye movements on hemispheric activity and choice of cognitive mode , 1978, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  R. Andrew,et al.  Right hemisphere advantage for topographical orientation in the domestic chick , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  R. Andrew The nature of behavioural lateralization in the chick , 1991 .

[26]  R. Andrew,et al.  Simultaneous changes in behaviour and in lateralization during the development of male and female domestic chicks , 1989, Animal Behaviour.

[27]  J. Coney,et al.  The effect of retention interval upon hemispheric processes in recognition memory , 1988, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  Hemispheric asymmetry of learning-induced changes , 1991 .

[29]  L. Rogers Lateralization of Learning in Chicks , 1986 .

[30]  O. Güntürkün Avian visual lateralization: a review. , 1997, Neuroreport.

[31]  S. Rose,et al.  The Making of Memory: From Molecules to Mind , 1996 .

[32]  R. J. Andrew,et al.  Age- and stimulus-specific use of right and left eyes by the domestic chick , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[33]  Ruben C. Gur,et al.  15 – Correlates of Conjugate Lateral Eye Movements in Man , 1977 .

[34]  G. Vallortigara,et al.  Lateralization of detour behaviour in poeciliid fish: The effect of species, gender and sexual motivation , 1998, Behavioural Brain Research.

[35]  R. Andrew,et al.  The lateralization of fear behaviour in the male domestic chick: A developmental study , 1983, Animal Behaviour.

[36]  G. Horn,et al.  Development of filial preferences in dark-reared chicks , 1988, Animal Behaviour.

[37]  G. Vallortigara Behavioral asymmetries in visual learning of young chickens , 1989, Physiology & Behavior.

[38]  R. Andrew Lateralization of Emotional and Cognitive Function in Higher Vertebrates, with Special Reference to the Domestic Chick , 1983 .

[39]  Lesley J. Rogers,et al.  Development of lateralization , 1991 .

[40]  Á. Miklósi,et al.  Auditory lateralisation: shifts in ear use during attachment in the domestic chick. , 1996, Laterality.

[41]  A. Deckel Laterality of aggressive responses in Anolis , 1995 .

[42]  G. Vallortigara,et al.  Lateralization of response by chicks to change in a model partner , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[43]  R. Andrew,et al.  Asymmetries of eye use in birds , 1986, Animal Behaviour.

[44]  Giorgio Vallortigara,et al.  Right hemisphere advantage for social recognition in the chick , 1992, Neuropsychologia.

[45]  G. Horn,et al.  Neural bases of recognition memory investigated through an analysis of imprinting. , 1990, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[46]  G. Vallortigara,et al.  Right-left asymmetry in position learning of male chicks , 1988, Behavioural Brain Research.

[47]  Lesley J. Rogers,et al.  Development of Brain and Behaviour in the Chicken , 1995 .

[48]  L. Rogers,et al.  Behavioral, Structural and Neurochemical Asymmetries in the Avian Brain: A Model System for Studying Visual Development and Processing , 1996, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[49]  L. Rogers Early experiential effects on laterality: research on chicks has relevance to other species. , 1997, Laterality.

[50]  G. Vallortigara,et al.  Lateral asymmetries due to preferences in eye use during visual discrimination learning in chicks , 1996, Behavioural Brain Research.