Priming randomness increases the evaluation of ritual efficacy

Reestablishing feelings of control after experiencing uncertainty has long been considered a fundamental motive for human behavior. We propose that rituals (i.e., socially stipulated, causally opaque practices) provide a means for coping with the aversive feelings associated with randomness due to the perception of a connection between ritual action and a desired outcome. Two experiments were conducted (one in Brazil [n = 40] and another in the United States [n = 94]) to evaluate how the perceived efficacy of rituals is affected by feelings of randomness. In a between-subjects design, the Scramble Sentence Task was used as a priming procedure in three conditions (i.e., randomness, negativity, and neutral) and participants were then asked to rate the efficacy of rituals used for problem-solving purposes. The results demonstrate that priming randomness increased participants' perception of ritual efficacy relative to negativity and neutral conditions. Implications for increasing our understanding of the relationship between perceived control and ritualistic behavior are discussed.

[1]  T. Ciborowski,et al.  "Superstition" in the collegiate baseball player. , 1997 .

[2]  Grzegorz Sedek,et al.  Personal Control in Action , 1998 .

[3]  Leigh Ann Vaughn,et al.  Causal uncertainty and correction of judgments , 2003 .

[4]  Jerry M. Lewis,et al.  Rationalists, Fatalists, and the Modern Superstitious: Test-Taking in Introductory Sociology , 2001 .

[5]  Grzegorz Sedek,et al.  Personal control in action : cognitive and motivational mechanisms , 1998 .

[6]  Edward A. Wasserman,et al.  Preliminaries to free throw shooting: Superstitious behavior? , 1986 .

[7]  C. Frederick,et al.  Superstitious behavior in sport: Levels of effectiveness and determinants of use in three collegiate sports. , 1998 .

[8]  Aaron C. Kay,et al.  God and the government: testing a compensatory control mechanism for the support of external systems. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[9]  Giora Keinan,et al.  The Effects of Stress and Desire for Control on Superstitious Behavior , 2002 .

[10]  S. Vyse Believing in Magic: The Psychology of Superstition , 1997 .

[11]  Lyn Y. Abramson,et al.  Induced mood and the illusion of control. , 1981 .

[12]  Adam D. Galinsky,et al.  Lacking Control Increases Illusory Pattern Perception , 2008, Science.

[13]  Michael W. Bridges,et al.  Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): a reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[14]  G. Keinan Effects of stress and tolerance of ambiguity on magical thinking. , 1994 .

[15]  Rapoport Jl,et al.  Recent advances in obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 1991 .

[16]  J. Rudski,et al.  Malinowski Goes to College: Factors Influencing Students' Use of Ritual and Superstition , 2007, The Journal of general psychology.

[17]  G. Weary,et al.  Individual differences in causal uncertainty. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[18]  P. Boyer,et al.  Why ritualized behavior? Precaution Systems and action parsing in developmental, pathological and cultural rituals , 2006, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[19]  M. Bradley,et al.  Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW): Instruction Manual and Affective Ratings , 1999 .

[20]  Harvey Whitehouse,et al.  Transmissive Frequency, Ritual, and Exegesis , 2001 .

[21]  G. Weary,et al.  Chronic and temporarily activated causal uncertainty beliefs and stereotype usage. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[22]  Frederick M. Smith,et al.  The Archetypal Actions of Ritual: A Theory of Ritual Illustrated by the Jain Rite of Worship , 1994 .

[23]  Helena Matute,et al.  Learned Helplessness and Superstitious Behavior as Opposite Effects of Uncontrollable Reinforcement in Humans , 1994 .

[24]  Britton W. Brewer,et al.  Chance orientation and superstitious behaviour on the putting green. , 1991 .

[25]  Cristine H. Legare,et al.  Cognitive consequences and constraints on reasoning about ritual , 2013 .

[26]  Jill A. Jacobson,et al.  Causal uncertainty beliefs and diagnostic information seeking. , 1997 .

[27]  Robert N. McCauley,et al.  Bringing ritual to mind : psychological foundations of cultural forms , 2002 .

[28]  S. Thompson,et al.  Illusions of control, underestimations, and accuracy: a control heuristic explanation. , 1998, Psychological bulletin.

[29]  B. Malinowski,et al.  Magic, science and religion : and other essays , 1948 .

[30]  Pascal Boyer,et al.  Precaution systems and ritualized behavior , 2006, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[31]  Cristine H. Legare,et al.  The role of testimony in the evaluation of religious expertise , 2011 .

[32]  Kristin Laurin,et al.  Randomness, Attributions of Arousal, and Belief in God , 2010, Psychological science.

[33]  G. Weary,et al.  Immediate and delayed effects of causal uncertainty inductions on uncertainty accessibility , 2008 .

[34]  Cristine H. Legare,et al.  Evaluating ritual efficacy: Evidence from the supernatural , 2012, Cognition.

[35]  A. Fiske,et al.  Is obsessive-compulsive disorder a pathology of the human disposition to perform socially meaningful rituals? Evidence of similar content. , 1997, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.

[36]  J L Rapoport,et al.  Recent advances in obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 1991, Neuropsychopharmacology : official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology.

[37]  Adam D. Galinsky,et al.  Illusory Control , 2008, Psychological science.

[38]  Birf Pesquisa Nacional por amostra de domicílios , 2003 .

[39]  Melissa Todd,et al.  Characteristics Associated with Superstitious Behavior in Track and Field Athletes: Are There NCAA Divisional Level Differences? , 2003 .

[40]  J. Rudski,et al.  Competition, superstition and the illusion of control , 2001 .

[41]  Miguel A. Vadillo,et al.  Illusion of Control , 2013, Experimental psychology.