Short-term immune challenge does not influence social dominance behaviour in top-ranked black-capped chickadees

In social species, groups of individuals that live, forage or interact with each other often form a linear dominance hierarchy. Dominant individuals are thought to have greater access to resources and the ability to obtain higher-quality territories or mates, making dominance rank important to maximizing fitness. Social animals are often susceptible to infections from parasites or pathogens, and the energetic cost of mounting an immune response, along with displaying sickness behaviours, could affect an individual's social relationships. If a dominant individual becomes sick and is made vulnerable to challenges from lower-ranking group members, a loss in rank could be detrimental. Many songbirds, such as the black-capped chickadee, Poecile atricapillus, form seasonal social flocks during the winter, then divide into pairs for breeding during the spring. Our study used captive chickadees to determine whether a challenge to the immune system affects social dominance behaviour and status in the winter flock. In captive experimental flocks, top-ranked individuals were given an immune challenge in the form of a lipopolysaccharide (LPS) injection, and dominance behaviour was observed to determine whether status was altered. LPS-injected individuals lost a significant amount of weight and increased feeding rate when compared to control-injected birds, indicating an energetic cost, but they showed no change in dominance behaviour or loss of rank. These findings suggest that maintaining rank by continuing to invest in dominance behaviours is important in order to retain the fitness benefits associated with a high dominance rank.

[1]  M. Zuk,et al.  Social environment and immunity in male red jungle fowl , 2000 .

[2]  S. M. Ramsay,et al.  Patterns of extrapair mating in relation to male dominance status and female nest placement in black-capped chickadees , 2004 .

[3]  Mary Poss,et al.  Social Organization and Parasite Risk in Mammals: Integrating Theory and Empirical Studies , 2003 .

[4]  A. Møller,et al.  Parasites and the Evolution of Host Social Behavior , 1993 .

[5]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[6]  J. Wingfield,et al.  Hormonal, behavioral, and thermoregulatory responses to bacterial lipopolysaccharide in captive and free-living white-crowned sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophrys gambelii) , 2006, Hormones and Behavior.

[7]  Kathryn J. Schneider Dominance, predation, and optimal foraging in white-throated sparrow flocks , 1984 .

[8]  J. Lockie THE FOOD AND FEEDING BEHAVIOUR OF THE JACKDAW, ROOK AND CARRION CROW , 1956 .

[9]  M. Zuk,et al.  Parasites influence social rank and morphology, but not mate choice, in female red junglefowl, Gallus gallus , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[10]  Rebecca M. Calisi,et al.  Lab and field experiments: Are they the same animal? , 2009, Hormones and Behavior.

[11]  P. Marra,et al.  West Nile virus emergence and large-scale declines of North American bird populations , 2007, Nature.

[12]  S. M. Ramsay,et al.  Variation in social rank acquisition influences lifetime reproductive success in black-capped chickadees , 2007 .

[13]  J. Wingfield,et al.  Social context modulates sickness behavior , 2012, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[14]  Lynn B. Martin,et al.  Population differences in fever and sickness behaviors in a wild passerine: a role for cytokines , 2010, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[15]  B. Hart Biological basis of the behavior of sick animals , 1988, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[16]  J. Adelman,et al.  Repeated stressors in adulthood increase the rate of biological ageing , 2015, Frontiers in Zoology.

[17]  D. Maestripieri,et al.  Male quality, dominance rank, and mating success in free-ranging rhesus macaques , 2015 .

[18]  A. Gosler On the use of greater covert moult and pectoral muscle as measures of condition in passerines with data for the Great Tit Parus major , 1991 .

[19]  K. Norris,et al.  Ecological immunology: life history trade-offs and immune defense in birds , 2000 .

[20]  R. Dantzer,et al.  Sickness behavior as a new target for drug development. , 1992, Trends in pharmacological sciences.

[21]  R. Yirmiya,et al.  The Immunobiology of Sexual Behavior Gender Differences in the Suppression of Sexual Activity During Illness , 1999, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[22]  P. Boag,et al.  Do female black-capped chickadees prefer high-ranking males as extra-pair partners? , 1998, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[23]  R. Dantzer,et al.  Cytokine-induced sickness behavior , 2003, Brain, Behavior, and Immunity.

[24]  D. Kinoshita,et al.  Lipopolysaccharide administration in the dominant mouse destabilizes social hierarchy , 2012, Behavioural Processes.

[25]  L. C. Sá-Rocha,et al.  Differential effects of lipopolysaccharide in the social behavior of dominant and submissive mice , 2006, Physiology & Behavior.

[26]  Susan M. Smith The Black-Capped Chickadee: Behavioral Ecology and Natural History , 1992 .

[27]  Social status in relation to Sindbis virus infection clearance in greenfinches , 2003, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[28]  B. Sheldon,et al.  Ecological immunology: costly parasite defences and trade-offs in evolutionary ecology. , 1996, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[29]  K. Dixon Dominance-Subordination Relationships in Mountain Chickadees , 1965 .

[30]  J. Adelman,et al.  Interactions between social behaviour and the acute phase immune response in house finches , 2015 .

[31]  W. J. Freeland Parasitism and behavioral dominance among male mice. , 1981, Science.

[32]  André A. Dhondt,et al.  Pathogen resistance and immunocompetence covary with social status in house finches (Carpodacus mexicanus) , 2007 .