On the comprehension of active and passive sentences

Abstract In a series of five experiments the ease of processing active and passive sentences was shown to be a function of the prior coding of a perceptual event. When the event was coded in terms of the actor, active sentences were more easily verified, when the event was coded in terms of the receiver of the action, passive sentences were more easily verified. This same pattern was shown to hold for answering active and passive questions. From this it was inferred that a passive sentence can be comprehended directly in the logical object-verb-logical subject word order without recovering its active sentence equivalent base structure. A processing model for the verification of active and passive sentences was proposed in terms of a series of binary comparison operations each of which requires additional time; this model was shown to account for about 90% of the variance in the time Ss required to verify these sentences.

[1]  P. Johnson-Laird The choice of the passive voice in a communicative task. , 1968, British journal of psychology.

[2]  Philip N. Johnson-Laird,et al.  Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Shorter Articles and Notes the Interpretation of the Passive Voice Shorter Articles and Notes the Interpretation of the Passive Voice , 2022 .

[3]  H. Savin,et al.  Grammatical structure and the immediate recall of english sentences , 1965 .

[4]  George A. Miller,et al.  Introduction to the Formal Analysis of Natural Languages , 1968 .

[5]  J. Huttenlocher,et al.  Comprehension and a statement's relation to the situation it describes , 1968 .

[6]  Peter Herriot,et al.  The comprehension of active and passive sentences as a function of pragmatic expectations , 1969 .

[7]  D R Olson,et al.  Language and thought: aspects of a cognitive theory of semantics. , 1970, Psychological review.

[8]  J. Fodor,et al.  Some syntactic determinants of sentential complexity , 1967 .

[9]  T. Trabasso,et al.  Storage and verification stages in processing concepts. , 1971 .

[10]  Philip B. Gough,et al.  Grammatical transformations and speed of understanding , 1965 .

[11]  J. Sachs Recognition memory for syntactic and semantic aspects of connected discourse , 1967 .

[12]  Herbert H. Clark,et al.  Linguistic processes in deductive reasoning. , 1969 .

[13]  P. Tannenbaum,et al.  Generation of active and passive sentences as a function of subject or object focus , 1968 .

[14]  J. Huttenlocher,et al.  Comprehension: Relation between perceived actor and logical subject , 1968 .

[15]  Philip B. Gough,et al.  The verification of sentences: The effects of delay of evidence and sentence length , 1966 .

[16]  P Wright,et al.  Transformations and the Understanding of Sentences , 1969, Language and speech.

[17]  W. R. Garner To perceive is to know. , 1966 .

[18]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Mental operations in the comparison of sentences and pictures. , 1972 .

[19]  J. Hayes Cognition and the development of language , 1970 .

[20]  W. C. Watt,et al.  On two hypotheses concerning psycholinguistics , 1969 .

[21]  Joseph H. Greenberg,et al.  Current trends in linguistics. , 1959, Science.

[22]  R. L. Dominowski Anagram solving as a function of letter moves , 1966 .

[23]  Eugene Galanter,et al.  Handbook of mathematical psychology: I. , 1963 .

[24]  J. Mehler Some effects of grammatical transformations on the recall of english sentences , 1963 .

[25]  D. Slobin Grammatical transformations and sentence comprehension in childhood and adulthood , 1966 .

[26]  P. Wason The contexts of plausible denial , 1965 .

[27]  Patricia Wright,et al.  Some observations on how people answer questions about sentences , 1972 .

[28]  C. Osgood Where do sentences come from , 1971 .

[29]  N. Goodman Languages of Art , 1968 .

[30]  G. Bower,et al.  Cognition in learning and memory , 1974 .