Adaptations to Transformations of the Optic Array

People adapt to visual distortions that are experienced, for instance, when they first wear spectacles. The changes underlying adaptation can occur in the oculocentriC., or retinotopiC., system, which codes the positions and movements of images on the retina. They can occur in the headcentric system, which codes the positions of objects with respect to the head, or in the body centric system, the system that allows us to point to a seen object with an unseen hand. Finally, they can occur in the exocentric system, which allows us to judge the position of an object with respect to an external frame of reference, for instance, the orientation of a line relative to vertical.

[1]  Ian P. Howard,et al.  Human visual orientation , 1982 .

[2]  I P Howard,et al.  Satiation and the tilt after-effect. , 1965, The American journal of psychology.

[3]  D. Regan,et al.  Human brain electrophysiology , 1989 .

[4]  Richard Held,et al.  Motor-Sensory Feedback and the Geometry of Visual Space , 1963, Science.

[5]  Gerald Westheimer Line-separation discrimination curve in the human fovea: smooth or segmented? , 1984 .

[6]  O. Favreau Persistence of simple and contingent motion aftereffects , 1979 .

[7]  Jeremy M. Wolfe,et al.  Short test flashes produce large tilt aftereffects , 1984, Vision Research.

[8]  I. Howard,et al.  Human Spatial Orientation , 1966 .

[9]  Irvin Rock Adaptation to a minified image , 1965 .

[10]  J. Loomis,et al.  Sensitivity to shifts of a point stimulus: An instance of tactile hyperacuity , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[11]  D. P. Andrews Perception of contour orientation in the central fovea part I: Short lines , 1967 .

[12]  D Regan,et al.  Spatial frequency discrimination in normal vision and in patients with multiple sclerosis. , 1982, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[13]  D. Regan,et al.  Illusory motion in depth: Aftereffect of adaptation to changing size , 1978, Vision Research.

[14]  I P Howard,et al.  Muscular and joint-receptor components in postural persistence. , 1974, Journal of experimental psychology.

[15]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Relative motion induced between stationary lines , 1978, Vision Research.

[16]  Hugh R. Wilson,et al.  Responses of spatial mechanisms can explain hyperacuity , 1986, Vision Research.

[17]  M J Morgan,et al.  Stimulus features that determine the visual location of a bright bar. , 1983, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[18]  K R Paap,et al.  Perceptual consequences of potentiation in the extraocular muscles: an alternative explanation for adaptation to wedge prisms. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  S. McKee,et al.  Precise velocity discrimination despite random variations in temporal frequency and contrast , 1986, Vision Research.

[20]  I. Rock,et al.  The effect of retinal and phenomenal orientation on the perception of form. , 1957, The American journal of psychology.

[21]  Scott N. J. Watamaniuk,et al.  Direction Perception in Complex Dynamic Displays: the Integration of Dir~~tion Information , 1988 .

[22]  I. Howard,et al.  The Effect of Steady Fixation on the Judgement of Relative Depth , 1964 .

[23]  J Hirsch,et al.  Limits of spatial-frequency discrimination as evidence of neural interpolation. , 1982, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[24]  D. Regan,et al.  Postadaptation orientation discrimination. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[25]  C. McCollough Color Adaptation of Edge-Detectors in the Human Visual System , 1965, Science.

[26]  G. Westheimer,et al.  Spatial location and hyperacuity: The centre/surround localization contribution function has two substrates , 1985, Vision Research.

[27]  M. Banks,et al.  The perception of verticality and the frame of reference of the visual tilt aftereffect , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  J M Wolfe,et al.  Fatigue and structural change: two consequences of visual pattern adaptation. , 1986, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[29]  M Jeannerod,et al.  An Attempt at Correlating Visuomotor-Induced Tilt Aftereffect and Ocular Cyclotorsion , 1981, Perception.

[30]  S. Klein,et al.  Hyperacuity thresholds of 1 sec: theoretical predictions and empirical validation. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[31]  C. L. Morgan Constancy of egocentric visual direction , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[32]  Adaptation to Optically Reduced Size , 1982, Perception.

[33]  H B Cohen Some critical factors in prism-adaptation. , 1966, The American journal of psychology.

[34]  S. F. Bowne Contrast discrimination cannot explain spatial frequency, orientation or temporal frequency discrimination , 1990, Vision Research.

[35]  D. Regan,et al.  Periodicity in orientation discrimination and the unconfounding of visual information , 1986, Vision Research.