Music is known to be an important facet of all human cultures (Merriam, 1964). Listening to music in order to influence moods, evoke strong emotions, and derive pleasure is becoming increasingly common, especially in this day and age when access to music is easy and quick. In recent years, exploring the neural correlates of musical emotions has attracted the attention of neuroscientists (Brattico & Pearce, 2013; Koelsch, Fritz, v. Cramon, Muller, & Friederici, 2006). However, the majority of these studies have not accounted for the effect of musical expertise, despite increasing evidence of structural and functional differences between musicians and nonmusicians, particularly in the regions of the cerebrum associated with motor, auditory, and visuospatial processing (Amunts et al., 1997; Angulo-Perkins et al., 2014; Baumann et al., 2007; Fauvel et al., 2014; Gaser & Schlaug, 2003; Herdener et al., 2010; Hutchinson, Lee, Gaab, & Schlaug, 2003; Hyde et al., 2009; Lee, Chen, & Schlaug, 2003; Pantev & Herholz, 2011; Schlaug, Jancke, Huang, Staiger, & Steinmetz, 1995; Schlaug, Jancke, Huang, & Steinmetz, 1995; Sluming, Taylor, Keller, Cezayirli, & Roberts, 2003; Wong, Skoe, Russo, Dees, & Kraus, 2007). In addition, musicians were found to have greater amounts of white matter in the anterior corpus callosum than nonmusicians (for a review, see Moore, Schaefer, Bastin, Roberts, & Overy, 2014). Furthermore, higher cerebellar volume is associated with male musicians than nonmusicians (Hutchinson et al., 2003; Sluming et al., 2003), particularly concerning the regions involved in representing movement (Gaser & Schlaug, 2003). In addition, musicians have been found to have greater sensitivity in processing and discriminating sounds in the auditory cortex (Musacchia, Strait, & Kraus, 2008; Pantev et al., 1998; Strait et al., 2009; Tervaniemi et al., 2001; Wong et al., 2007). In sum, long-term musical practice causes structural and functional changes in the brain, with subsequent enhanced representation of sounds and music (see Kraus & Chandrasekaran, 2010; Reybrouck & Brattico, 2015 for an overview).The neuroscientific studies on musical emotions have predominantly utilized the classical controlled paradigm wherein the condition of interest is interspersed with other tasks or baseline, thereby not providing the entire picture of emotional networks in the brain. Recent times have seen an increase in fMRI studies that employ the continuous listening paradigm, in which participants listen to music continuously, typically lasting the entire span of the piece, and without any intermittent task, for investigating neural processing of music (Abrams et al., 2013; Alluri et al., 2012, 2013; Burunat, Alluri, Toiviainen, Numminen, & Brattico, 2014; Toiviainen, Alluri, Brattico, Wallentin, & Vuust, 2014; Trost, Ethofer, Zentner, & Vuilleumier, 2012). However, those studies examined whole brain regional activations, not focusing on limbic system structures or on the modulation of neural activity and connectivity by musical expertise. A single fMRI study (Koelsch et al., 2013) has used full pieces of music to investigate the dynamics of emotion processing in the limbic system, albeit with participants without musical training.On the contrary, resting-state-based fMRI studies have dealt with investigating functional networks that emerge during prolonged periods of rest without any stimulus (Lee, Smyser, & Shimony, 2013). There is abundant literature examining restingstate connectivity and connectivity in the visual domain, especially in terms of decoding. However, there has been a scarcity of studies investigating functional connectivity in the auditory domain as a function of musical expertise. The first resting-state study investigating connectivity differences between musicians and nonmusicians in resting state was conducted by Luo et al. (2012). Their functional connectivity analysis was limited to five seed regions representing the right primary motor, left primary auditory, left primary somatosensory cortices, and the left primary visual and left V2 areas. …
[1]
J. Staiger,et al.
Increased corpus callosum size in musicians
,
1995,
Neuropsychologia.
[2]
Jonathan P. McNulty,et al.
The salience network is responsible for switching between the default mode network and the central executive network: Replication from DCM
,
2014,
NeuroImage.
[3]
Elvira Brattico,et al.
Cognitive and Emotional Modulation of Brain Default Operation
,
2009,
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[4]
Peter E. Keller,et al.
The Perception of Musical Spontaneity in Improvised and Imitated Jazz Performances
,
2011,
Front. Psychology.
[5]
R. Schaefer,et al.
Can Musical Training Influence Brain Connectivity? Evidence from Diffusion Tensor MRI
,
2014,
Brain sciences.
[6]
E. Brattico,et al.
Neuroplasticity beyond Sounds: Neural Adaptations Following Long-Term Musical Aesthetic Experiences
,
2015,
Brain sciences.
[7]
E. Brattico,et al.
Emotion Processing of Major, Minor, and Dissonant Chords
,
2005,
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[8]
R. Zatorre,et al.
Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emotion to music
,
2011,
Nature Neuroscience.
[9]
Alan C. Evans,et al.
Emotional responses to pleasant and unpleasant music correlate with activity in paralimbic brain regions
,
1999,
Nature Neuroscience.
[10]
Steven C. R. Williams,et al.
A functional MRI study of happy and sad affective states induced by classical music
,
2007,
Human brain mapping.
[11]
S. Koelsch.
Brain correlates of music-evoked emotions
,
2014,
Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[12]
A. Friederici,et al.
Investigating emotion with music: An fMRI study
,
2006,
Human brain mapping.
[13]
D. Grandjean,et al.
The role of the medial temporal limbic system in processing emotions in voice and music
,
2014,
Progress in Neurobiology.
[14]
Li Dong,et al.
Long-Term Effects of Musical Training and Functional Plasticity in Salience System
,
2014,
Neural plasticity.
[15]
M. Zentner,et al.
Mapping Aesthetic Musical Emotions in the Brain
,
2011,
Cerebral cortex.
[16]
M. Steen,et al.
ERRATUM: Network Science and the Effects of Music Preference on Functional Brain Connectivity: From Beethoven to Eminem
,
2014,
Scientific Reports.
[17]
Stefan Koelsch,et al.
The roles of superficial amygdala and auditory cortex in music-evoked fear and joy
,
2013,
NeuroImage.
[18]
Parag Chordia,et al.
Inter‐subject synchronization of brain responses during natural music listening
,
2013,
The European journal of neuroscience.
[19]
Fred L. Steinberg,et al.
Dynamic Emotional and Neural Responses to Music Depend on Performance Expression and Listener Experience
,
2010,
PloS one.
[20]
Niels Birbaumer,et al.
Overt and imagined singing of an Italian aria
,
2007,
NeuroImage.
[21]
N. Rickard.
Intense emotional responses to music: a test of the physiological arousal hypothesis
,
2004
.