Beyond replay: Introduction to the special issue on hippocampal replay

The hippocampus has at least two modes of operation: theta, which appears as a 6—10 Hz rhythm (in the rat) of repeating components with a 100—180 ms period, during which the hippocampal place fields sweep forward from a rat's current position ahead of the animal, and large amplitude irregular activity (LIA), which reveals a broader spectrum in the local field potential, and is punctuated by transient sharp wave—ripple complexes (SWRs, 150—200 ms bursts of coherent 150—200 Hz rhythms) and hippocampal replay events (Buzsáki, Leung, & Vanderwolf, 1983; Foster, 2017; O'Keefe & Nadel, 1978; Redish, 1999, 2016; Vanderwolf, 1969). These different modes have been seen in other animals as well from mice to bats to monkeys to humans (Buzsáki, 2015). When it was first observed that hippocampal cells “replayed” experiences by reactivating during rest and sleep following behavior (Nádasdy, Hirase, Czurkó, Csicsvari, & Buzsáki, 1999; Pavlides & Winson, 1989; Skaggs & McNaughton, 1996; Wilson & McNaughton, 1994), it was thought that replay was part of a consolidation process that wrote information from a transient memory storage in hippocampus to a long-term storage in cortex (see Sutherland & McNaughton, 2000; Redish, 1999 for reviews of the classic literature). While evidence still supports this original theory (Klinzing, Niethard, & Born, 2019), over the years, it has been found that these replays occur during waking states as well (Kudrimoti, Barnes, & McNaughton, 1999), differ between waking and sleeping states (Wikenheiser & Redish, 2013; Buzsáki, 2015), and change as animals become familiar with their environments (Cheng & Frank, 2008; Poe, Nitz, McNaughton, & Barnes, 2000; Shin, Tang, & Jadhav, 2019; Wu & Foster, 2014). Hippocampal representation during these “replay” events can show both forward and backward sequences (Davidson, Kloosterman, & Wilson, 2009; Foster & Wilson, 2006; Gupta, van der Meer, Touretzky, & Redish, 2010; Ólafsdóttir, Bush, & Barry, 2018; Shin et al., 2019), can reveal shortcuts the animal has never experienced (Dragoi & Tonegawa, 2013; Gupta et al., 2010; Ólafsdóttir, Barry, Saleem, Hassabis, & Spiers, 2015), and [sometimes] predict where the animal is going to go (Pfeiffer & Foster, 2013; Schmidt, Wikenheiser, & Redish, 2018; Shin et al., 2019; Singer, Carr, Karlsson, & Frank, 2013) but [sometimes] prefer where the animal has not been or is not going to go (Carey, Tanaka, & van der Meer, 2019; Gupta et al., 2010). Hippocampal replay has been found to interact with deep medial entorhinal cortex (Buzsáki, 2015; Chrobak & Buzsáki, 1996; Ólafsdóttir, Carpenter, & Barry, 2016), prelimbic cortex (Euston, Gruber, & McNaughton, 2012; Peyrache, Khamassi, Benchenane, Wiener, & Battaglia, 2009; Shin et al., 2019), as well as other cortical areas (Buzsáki, 2015). Hippocampal replay events are aligned with value representations in the ventral striatum (nucleus accumbens core, Lansink, Goltstein, Lankelma, McNaughton, & Pennartz, 2009). Functionally, disruption of hippocampal SWRs during sleep has been found to disrupt goal-related learning across days (Ego-Stengel & Wilson, 2010; Maingret, Girardeau, Todorova, Goutierre, & Zugaro, 2016), while enhancement of rewarding signals (medial forebrain bundle stimulation) during place cell activation during sleep creates goalrelated learning (de Lavilléon, Lacroix, Rondi-Reig, & Benchenane, 2015). However, disruption of hippocampal SWRs during waking states produces changes in the ability to use working memory to plan and take correct paths (Fernández-Ruiz et al., 2019; Jadhav, Kemere, German, & Frank, 2012). In this special issue, seven leaders in the field along with their teams present new data, reviews, and theoretical insights into the complexity that is hippocampal “replay.” We start with Pfeiffer (2020), who reviews the relationship of the content of replay to recent actions and future plans. Laventure and Benchenane (2020) review the evidence that sleep replay during SWRs really is causally involved in consolidation properties and in linking spatial signals with emotional valence. Cowen, Gray, Wiegand, Schimanski, and Barnes (2020) report interesting observations of the differences in waking SWR rates between young and aged animals, finding that younger animals showed higher rates, but that older animals emitted more ripples over the total time due to differences in their underlying behaviors. Todorova and Zugaro (2020) review the relation between hippocampal SWRs and extra-hippocampal neural structures, examining the relationship to other oscillations that occur during sleep and wake states. Hussin, Leonard, and Hoffman (2020) review the spatiotemporal features of SWRs in primates and compare their properties to behavioral effects in macaque monkeys. Zielinski, Tang, and Jadhav (2020) review the hippocampal-prefrontal interactions during theta and SWRs and suggest a theoretical complexity in their interactive roles as animals learn new tasks. Finally, Rusu and Pennartz (2020) provide a review placing SWRs in the context of large-scale theories of decision-making and behavior, suggesting that they play different roles in goal-directed versus habitual behaviors and comparing these roles to putative roles in contrasting theories of model-based and model-free learning, with a particular emphasis on the role of prefrontal-striatal loops and the relation to hippocampal SWRs. DOI: 10.1002/hipo.23184

[1]  Karim Benchenane,et al.  Validating the theoretical bases of sleep reactivation during sharp‐wave ripples and their association with emotional valence , 2020, Hippocampus.

[2]  G. Buzsáki,et al.  Long-duration hippocampal sharp wave ripples improve memory , 2019, Science.

[3]  G. Buzsáki,et al.  High-Frequency Oscillations in the Output Networks of the Hippocampal–Entorhinal Axis of the Freely Behaving Rat , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  Nachum Ulanovsky,et al.  Nonoscillatory Phase Coding and Synchronization in the Bat Hippocampal Formation , 2018, Cell.

[5]  J. Csicsvari,et al.  Replay and Time Compression of Recurring Spike Sequences in the Hippocampus , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  L. Frank,et al.  New Experiences Enhance Coordinated Neural Activity in the Hippocampus , 2008, Neuron.

[7]  M. Zugaro,et al.  Hippocampo-cortical coupling mediates memory consolidation during sleep , 2016, Nature Neuroscience.

[8]  D. R. Euston,et al.  The Role of Medial Prefrontal Cortex in Memory and Decision Making , 2012, Neuron.

[9]  Shantanu P. Jadhav,et al.  The role of replay and theta sequences in mediating hippocampal‐prefrontal interactions for memory and cognition , 2020, Hippocampus.

[10]  M. Wilson,et al.  Disruption of ripple‐associated hippocampal activity during rest impairs spatial learning in the rat , 2009, Hippocampus.

[11]  M. Khamassi,et al.  Replay of rule-learning related neural patterns in the prefrontal cortex during sleep , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  C. Pennartz,et al.  Learning, memory and consolidation mechanisms for behavioral control in hierarchically organized cortico‐basal ganglia systems , 2019, Hippocampus.

[13]  Stephen L. Cowen,et al.  Age‐associated changes in waking hippocampal sharp‐wave ripples , 2018, Hippocampus.

[14]  R. Passingham The hippocampus as a cognitive map J. O'Keefe & L. Nadel, Oxford University Press, Oxford (1978). 570 pp., £25.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.

[15]  G. Buzsáki,et al.  Cellular bases of hippocampal EEG in the behaving rat , 1983, Brain Research Reviews.

[16]  Margaret F. Carr,et al.  Hippocampal SWR Activity Predicts Correct Decisions during the Initial Learning of an Alternation Task , 2013, Neuron.

[17]  Bruce L. McNaughton,et al.  Experience-dependent phase-reversal of hippocampal neuron firing during REM sleep , 2000, Brain Research.

[18]  George Dragoi,et al.  Distinct preplay of multiple novel spatial experiences in the rat , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[19]  Jan Born,et al.  Mechanisms of systems memory consolidation during sleep , 2019, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  Andrew M. Wikenheiser,et al.  The balance of forward and backward hippocampal sequences shifts across behavioral states , 2013, Hippocampus.

[21]  Karim Benchenane,et al.  Explicit memory creation during sleep demonstrates a causal role of place cells in navigation , 2015, Nature Neuroscience.

[22]  C. H. Vanderwolf,et al.  Hippocampal electrical activity and voluntary movement in the rat. , 1969, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[23]  L. Frank,et al.  Awake Hippocampal Sharp-Wave Ripples Support Spatial Memory , 2012, Science.

[24]  Caswell Barry,et al.  Coordinated grid and place cell replay during rest , 2016, Nature Neuroscience.

[25]  A. Redish Beyond the Cognitive Map: From Place Cells to Episodic Memory , 1999 .

[26]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Replay of Neuronal Firing Sequences in Rat Hippocampus During Sleep Following Spatial Experience , 1996, Science.

[27]  David J Foster,et al.  Hippocampal Replay Captures the Unique Topological Structure of a Novel Environment , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Hippocampus Leads Ventral Striatum in Replay of Place-Reward Information , 2009, PLoS biology.

[29]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Memory trace reactivation in hippocampal and neocortical neuronal ensembles , 2000, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[30]  Matthew A. Wilson,et al.  Hippocampal Replay of Extended Experience , 2009, Neuron.

[31]  Matthijs A. A. van der Meer,et al.  Hippocampal Replay Is Not a Simple Function of Experience , 2010, Neuron.

[32]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. , 1994, Science.

[33]  G. Buzsáki Hippocampal sharp wave‐ripple: A cognitive biomarker for episodic memory and planning , 2015, Hippocampus.

[34]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Reactivation of Hippocampal Cell Assemblies: Effects of Behavioral State, Experience, and EEG Dynamics , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  Brad E. Pfeiffer,et al.  Hippocampal place cell sequences depict future paths to remembered goals , 2013, Nature.

[36]  Timothy K. Leonard,et al.  Sharp‐wave ripple features in macaques depend on behavioral state and cell‐type specific firing , 2018, Hippocampus.

[37]  Alyssa A. Carey,et al.  Reward revaluation biases hippocampal sequence content away from the preferred outcome , 2018, bioRxiv.

[38]  M. Zugaro,et al.  Hippocampal ripples as a mode of communication with cortical and subcortical areas , 2018, Hippocampus.

[39]  J. G. Taylor,et al.  Vicarious trial and error. , 1951, Psychological review.

[40]  David J. Foster,et al.  Reverse replay of behavioural sequences in hippocampal place cells during the awake state , 2006, Nature.

[41]  D. Hassabis,et al.  Hippocampal place cells construct reward related sequences through unexplored space , 2015, eLife.

[42]  David J. Foster Replay Comes of Age. , 2017, Annual review of neuroscience.

[43]  Brad E. Pfeiffer,et al.  The content of hippocampal “replay” , 2018, Hippocampus.

[44]  C. Pavlides,et al.  Influences of hippocampal place cell firing in the awake state on the activity of these cells during subsequent sleep episodes , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[45]  Wenbo Tang,et al.  Dynamics of Awake Hippocampal-Prefrontal Replay for Spatial Learning and Memory-Guided Decision Making , 2019, Neuron.

[46]  N. Ulanovsky,et al.  Hippocampal cellular and network activity in freely moving echolocating bats , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[47]  C. Barry,et al.  The Role of Hippocampal Replay in Memory and Planning , 2018, Current Biology.