Orienting of attention via observed eye gaze is head-centred

Observing averted eye gaze results in the automatic allocation of attention to the gazed-at location. The role of the orientation of the face that produces the gaze cue was investigated. The eyes in the face could look left or right in a head-centred frame, but the face itself could be oriented 90 degrees clockwise or anticlockwise such that the eyes were gazing up or down. Significant cueing effects to targets presented to the left or right of the screen were found in these head orientation conditions. This suggests that attention was directed to the side to which the eyes would have been looking towards, had the face been presented upright. This finding provides evidence that head orientation can affect gaze following, even when the head orientation alone is not a social cue. It also shows that the mechanism responsible for the allocation of attention following a gaze cue can be influenced by intrinsic object-based (i.e. head-centred) properties of the task-irrelevant cue.

[1]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Uninformative symbolic cues may bias visual-spatial attention: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence , 1997, Biological Psychology.

[2]  R. Yin Looking at Upside-down Faces , 1969 .

[3]  Alan Kingstone,et al.  Covert and overt orienting to gaze direction cues and the effects of fixation offset , 2003, Neuroreport.

[4]  J. Driver,et al.  Can Visual Neglect Operate in Object-centred Co-ordinates? An Affirmative Single-case Study , 1991 .

[5]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  Object-centered encoding by face-selective neurons in the cortex in the superior temporal sulcus of the monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  J. Tipples Eye gaze is not unique: Automatic orienting in response to uninformative arrows , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[7]  R. Proctor,et al.  Deconstructing Marilyn: Robust effects of face contexts on stimulus—response compatibility , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[8]  R Lawson,et al.  Achieving visual object constancy across plane rotation and depth rotation. , 1999, Acta psychologica.

[9]  G. Baird,et al.  Testing joint attention, imitation, and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind , 2000 .

[10]  J. Hietanen,et al.  Social attention orienting integrates visual information from head and body orientation , 2002, Psychological research.

[11]  D. Perrett,et al.  Being the target of another’s emotion: a PET study , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  Marlene Behrmann,et al.  Object-centered not scene-based visual neglect. , 1996 .

[13]  B. Hommel,et al.  S-R compatibility effects due to context-dependent spatial stimulus coding , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[14]  D. Perrett,et al.  Neural Representation for the Perception of the Intentionality of Actions , 2000, Brain and Cognition.

[15]  S. Langton The Mutual Influence of Gaze and Head Orientation in the Analysis of Social Attention Direction , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[16]  A. J. Mistlin,et al.  Visual cells in the temporal cortex sensitive to face view and gaze direction , 1985, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[17]  J. Findlay,et al.  The Relationship between Eye Movements and Spatial Attention , 1986, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[18]  A. Kingstone,et al.  Are eyes special? It depends on how you look at it , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[19]  J. Decety,et al.  Brain Regions Involved in the Perception of Gaze: A PET Study , 1998, NeuroImage.

[20]  J. Gibson,et al.  Perception of another person's looking behavior. , 1963, The American journal of psychology.

[21]  J. Bartlett,et al.  Inversion and Configuration of Faces , 1993, Cognitive Psychology.

[22]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Gaze Perception Triggers Reflexive Visuospatial Orienting , 1999 .

[23]  M. Gazzaniga,et al.  Reflexive Joint Attention Depends on Lateralized Cortical Connections , 2000, Psychological science.

[24]  V. Bruce,et al.  Reflexive visual orienting in response to the social attention of others , 1999 .

[25]  D I Perrett,et al.  Organization and functions of cells responsive to faces in the temporal cortex. , 1992, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[26]  A. Kingstone,et al.  The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze , 1998 .

[27]  N. Emery,et al.  The eyes have it: the neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze , 2000, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[28]  C. Moore,et al.  The role of movement in the development of joint visual attention , 1997 .

[29]  J. Haxby,et al.  Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[30]  S. Baron-Cohen The Eye Direction Detector (EDD) and the Shared Attention Mechanism (SAM): Two cases for evolutionar , 1995 .

[31]  C. Moore,et al.  Joint attention : its origins and role in development , 1995 .

[32]  V. Bruce,et al.  Do the eyes have it? Cues to the direction of social attention , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[33]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Is There a "Language of the Eyes"? Evidence from Normal Adults, and Adults with Autism or Asperger Syndrome , 1997 .

[34]  S. Tipper,et al.  Object-centered not scene-based visual neglect. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[35]  A. Young,et al.  Neglect and visual recognition. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[36]  A. Young,et al.  Reading the mind from eye gaze , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  J. Hietanen Does your gaze direction and head orientation shift my visual attention? , 1999, Neuroreport.

[38]  A. J. Mistlin,et al.  Visual analysis of body movements by neurones in the temporal cortex of the macaque monkey: A preliminary report , 1985, Behavioural Brain Research.