The Assembly of Phonology in Italian and English: Consonants and Vowels

This chapter is focussed on the nature of the assembly mechanism in reading. Several topics are discussed, but the main issue is related to whether independent representations and processes are involved in the construction of the phonology for consonants and vowels. The model proposed by Berent and Perfetti (1995), the two-cycles model, supporting the notion of a processing priority for consonants, is presented and discussed in the light of recent experimental data in Italian, showing a pattern that provides only partial support for the two-cycles model. In particular, the processing advantage for consonants as compared to vowels appearing in the backward masking paradigm in Berent and Perfetti’s data can be attributed to the irregularity of vowels in the English language. Another factor that may affect the process of converting print into sound, and is important in Italian, is the ratio between consonants and vowels. Because there are fewer vowels than consonants in Italian, they belong to a smaller domain, and the phonological emergence of vowels is faster as compared to that of consonants, leading to their advantage in Italian. The implications of the different findings in Italian and English are discussed.

[1]  Ronald W. Noel,et al.  Chapter 15 Dual-route Models of Print to Sound: Red Herrings and Real Horses , 1992 .

[2]  C. Perfetti,et al.  Reading in English and Chinese: Evidence for a "universal" phonological principle. , 1992 .

[3]  J. Grainger,et al.  Phonology and Orthography in Visual Word Recognition: Evidence from Masked Non-Word Priming , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[4]  Charles A. Perfetti,et al.  Automatic (prelexical) phonetic activation in silent word reading: Evidence from backward masking*1 , 1988 .

[5]  Diana Archangeli,et al.  Yokuts harmony: evidence for doplanar representation in nonlinear phonology , 1985 .

[6]  D. Balota,et al.  Priming and attentional control of lexical and sublexical pathways during naming. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[7]  G. C. Orden A ROWS is a ROSE: Spelling, sound, and reading , 1987 .

[8]  Colin M. Macleod Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[9]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .

[10]  S. Lupker,et al.  Strategic Control in a Naming Task: Changing Routes or Changing Deadlines? , 1997 .

[11]  Veronika Coltheart,et al.  Phonological processes in reading: A tutorial review. , 1987 .

[12]  M. Coltheart Lexical access in simple reading tasks , 1978 .

[13]  R. Cubelli A selective deficit for writing vowels in acquired dysgraphia , 1991, Nature.

[14]  I. Berent Phonological priming in the lexical decision task: regularity effects are not necessary evidence for assembly. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  C. Perfetti,et al.  VERY EARLY PHONOLOGICAL ACTIVATION IN CHINESE READING , 1995 .

[16]  Paul W. B. Atkins,et al.  Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. , 1993 .

[17]  Michael C. Doyle,et al.  Effects of frequency on visual word recognition tasks: where are they? , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[18]  P. Naish The Effects of Graphemic and Phonemic Similarity between Targets and Masks in a Backward Visual Masking Paradigm , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[19]  John J. McCarthy,et al.  Linear order in phonological representation , 1989 .

[20]  J Grainger,et al.  Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: a multiple read-out model. , 1996, Psychological review.

[21]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  When does irregular spelling or pronunciation influence word recognition , 1984 .

[22]  Patrizia Tabossi,et al.  Chapter 16 Strategies and Stress Assignment: Evidence from a Shallow Orthography , 1992 .

[23]  D. Besner,et al.  Visual Word Recognition: Evidence for Strategic Control of Lexical and Nonlexical Routines in Oral Reading , 1991 .

[24]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Spelling-sound effects in reading: Time-course and decision criteria , 1985, Memory & cognition.

[25]  Lucia Colombo,et al.  Interacting sources of information in word naming: a study of individual differences , 1994 .

[26]  Andrew Olson,et al.  Are Phonemic Effects in Backward Masking Evidence for Automatic Prelexical Phonemic Activation in Visual Word Recognition , 1995 .

[27]  David C. Plaut,et al.  Structure and Function in the Lexical System: Insights from Distributed Models of Word Reading and Lexical Decision , 1997 .

[28]  Daniel G Bobrow,et al.  On data-limited and resource-limited processes , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[29]  P. Tabossi,et al.  Semantic priming in the pronunciation of words in two writing systems: Italian and English , 1992, Memory & cognition.

[30]  Derek Besner,et al.  The assembly of phonology in oral reading: A new model. , 1987 .

[31]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: part 1.: an account of basic findings , 1988 .

[32]  Bruce F. Pennington,et al.  Word identification in reading and the promise of subsymbolic psycholinguistics. , 1990, Psychological review.

[33]  Alvin M. Liberman,et al.  The Grammars of Speech and Language. , 1970 .

[34]  S. Lupker,et al.  Effects of memory load in a word-naming task: Five failures to replicate , 1995, Memory & cognition.

[35]  M. Zorzi,et al.  Two routes or one in reading aloud? A connectionist dual-process model. , 1998 .

[36]  S. Monsell,et al.  Lexical and sublexical translation of spelling to sound : Strategic anticipation of lexical status , 1992 .

[37]  George A. Miller,et al.  The Reconstruction of Mutilated English Texts , 1957, Inf. Control..

[38]  C. Perfetti,et al.  A rose is a REEZ: The two-cycles model of phonology assembly in reading English , 1995 .

[39]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.

[40]  James L. McClelland,et al.  On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. , 1990, Psychological review.

[41]  M. Adams What good is orthographic redundancy , 1980 .

[42]  Mark S. Seidenberg The time course of phonological code activation in two writing systems , 1985, Cognition.

[43]  J Grainger,et al.  Effects of Orthography are Independent of Phonology in Masked form Priming , 1994, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[44]  C. Perfetti,et al.  Phonemic activation during the first 40 ms of word identification: Evidence from backward masking and priming , 1991 .

[45]  T. Carr,et al.  DUAL-ROUTE THEORIES OF PRONOUNCING PRINTED WORDS : WHAT CAN BE LEARNED FROM CONCURRENT TASK PERFORMANCE ? , 1996 .

[46]  H. Hawkins,et al.  Flexible coding in word recognition. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[47]  K. Paap,et al.  Dual-route models of print to sound: Still a good horse race , 1991 .

[48]  M. Turvey,et al.  Automatic and pre-lexical computation of phonology in visual word identification , 1990 .

[49]  L. Katz,et al.  Evidence of flexible coding in visual word recognition. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.